Inhibiting natural knowledge in fourth graders: towards a comprehensive test instrument

While understanding rational numbers forms an essential part of mathematical literacy, research has repeatedly shown that they form a stumbling block in education. A big source of difficulty is the inappropriate application of natural number knowledge. The research literature points at three main aspects where natural number knowledge is inappropriately applied: density, operations, and size. While there are numerous test instruments to assess each of these aspects, none of them combines all aspects in an integrated manner. By creating such a comprehensive test instrument and applying it to fourth graders, we found that the strength of the natural number bias was largest in density items, but still clearly present in items about size and operations. We further found that fourth graders’ rational number reasoning—more specifically their ability to inhibit the inappropriate use of natural number knowledge—can be described by a one-dimensional construct.

[1]  Thomas R. Post,et al.  Initial Fraction Learning by Fourth- and Fifth-Grade Students: A Comparison of the Effects of Using Commercial Curricula with the Effects of Using the Rational Number Project Curriculum. , 2002 .

[2]  S. Vosniadou,et al.  Understanding the structure of the set of rational numbers: a conceptual change approach , 2004 .

[3]  Klaus Hasemann On difficulties with fractions , 1981 .

[4]  Erno Lehtinen,et al.  Number concept and conceptual change: towards a systemic model of the processes of change , 2004 .

[5]  S. Vosniadou Capturing and modeling the process of conceptual change. , 1994 .

[6]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Naturally Biased? In Search for Reaction Time Evidence for a Natural Number Bias in Adults. , 2012 .

[7]  S. Vosniadou,et al.  How Many Decimals Are There Between Two Fractions? Aspects of Secondary School Students’ Understanding of Rational Numbers and Their Notation , 2010 .

[8]  John D. Bransford,et al.  How Students Learn: Mathematics in the Classroom. , 2005 .

[9]  Ron Hoz The effects of rigidity on school geometry learning , 1981 .

[10]  Ronli Diakow,et al.  Towards curricular coherence in integers and fractions: a study of the efficacy of a lesson sequence that uses the number line as the principal representational context , 2013 .

[11]  B. Greer The growth of mathematics through conceptual restructuring , 2004 .

[12]  Robert S Siegler,et al.  Early predictors of middle school fraction knowledge. , 2014, Developmental science.

[13]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Are secondary school students still hampered by the natural number bias? A reaction time study on fraction comparison tasks , 2013 .

[14]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  Dual Processes in the Psychology of Mathematics Education and Cognitive Psychology , 2009, Human Development.

[15]  Peter Bryant,et al.  Rational Numbers and Intensive Quantities: Challenges and Insights to Pupils´ Implicit Knowledge , 2008 .

[16]  T. Nunes,et al.  THE EQUIVALENCE AND ORDERING OF FRACTIONS IN PART-WHOLE AND QUOTIENT SITUATIONS Ema Mamede , 2006 .

[17]  L. Resnick,et al.  Conceptual Bases of Arithmetic Errors: The Case of Decimal Fractions. , 1989 .

[18]  Song A. An,et al.  Conceptualizing and organizing content for teaching and learning in selected Chinese, Japanese and US mathematics textbooks: the case of fraction division , 2009 .

[19]  M. Mazzocco,et al.  Parts and 'holes': gaps in rational number sense among children with vs. without mathematical learning disabilities. , 2008, Developmental science.

[20]  Georg Rasch,et al.  Probabilistic Models for Some Intelligence and Attainment Tests , 1981, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[21]  Susan Carey,et al.  Never getting to zero: Elementary school students’ understanding of the infinite divisibility of number and matter , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.

[22]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  The epigenesis of mathematical thinking , 2000 .

[23]  Yujing Ni,et al.  Teaching and Learning Fraction and Rational Numbers: The Origins and Implications of Whole Number Bias , 2005 .

[24]  Konstantinos P. Christou Natural number bias in operations with missing numbers , 2015 .

[25]  R. Siegler,et al.  Early Predictors of High School Mathematics Achievement , 2012, Psychological science.

[26]  Andreas Obersteiner,et al.  The natural number bias and magnitude representation in fraction comparison by expert mathematicians , 2013 .

[27]  S. Zeger,et al.  Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models , 1986 .

[28]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  In search for the natural number bias in secondary school students' interpretation of the effect of arithmetical operations , 2015 .

[29]  Wim Van Dooren,et al.  What Fills the Gap between Discrete and Dense? Greek and Flemish Students' Understanding of Density. , 2011 .

[30]  S. Embretson,et al.  Item response theory for psychologists , 2000 .

[31]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Searching for a whole number bias in secondary school students - a reaction time study on fraction comparison , 2012 .

[32]  S. Vosniadou The Framework Theory Approach to the Problem of Conceptual Change , 2009 .

[33]  E. Fischbein,et al.  THE ROLE OF IMPLICIT MODELS IN SOLVING VERBAL PROBLEMS IN MULTIPLICATION AND DIVISION , 1985 .

[34]  Stella Vosniadou,et al.  International handbook of research on conceptual change , 2013 .

[35]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Comparing the magnitude of two fractions with common components: which representations are used by 10- and 12-year-olds? , 2010, Journal of experimental child psychology.