Real-world stereoscopic performance in multiple-focal-plane displays: How far apart should the image planes be?

Conventional stereoscopic displays present conflicting stimuli to vergence and accommodation, causing fatigue, discomfort, and poor stereo depth perception. One promising solution is 'depth filtering', in which continuous variations in focal distance are simulated by distributing image intensity across multiple focal planes. The required image-plane spacing is a critical parameter, because there are constraints on the total number that can be used. Depth-filtered images have been shown to support continuous and reasonably accurate accommodation responses with 1.1 dioptre (D) image-plane spacings. However, retinal contrast is increasingly attenuated with increasing image-plane separation. Thus, while such stimuli may eliminate the vergence-accommodation conflict, they may also unacceptably degrade stereoscopic depth perception. Here we measured stereoacuity, and the time needed for stereoscopic fusion, for real targets and depth-filtered approximations to the same stimuli (image-plane spacings of 0.6, 0.9 and 1.2 D). Stereo fusion time was reasonably consistent across conditions. Stereoacuity for depth-filtered stimuli was only slightly poorer than for real targets with 0.6 D image-plane separation, but deteriorated rapidly thereafter. Our results suggest that stereoscopic depth perception, not accommodation and vergence responses, is the limiting factor in determining acceptable image-plane spacing for depth-filtered images. We suggest that image-plane spacing should ideally not exceed ~0.6 D.

[1]  Masaki Emoto,et al.  Two factors in visual fatigue caused by stereoscopic HDTV images , 2004 .

[2]  Martin S. Banks,et al.  Achieving near-correct focus cues in a 3D display using multiple image planes , 2005, IS&T/SPIE Electronic Imaging.

[3]  Felix Wichmann,et al.  The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  K N OGLE,et al.  Observations on accommodative convergence; especially its nonlinear relationships. , 1959, American journal of ophthalmology.

[5]  D. A. Owens A comparison of accommodative responsiveness and contrast sensitivity for sinusoidal gratings , 1980, Vision Research.

[6]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[7]  P. Ward,et al.  THE EFFECT OF SPATIAL FREQUENCY ON STEADY‐STATE ACCOMMODATION , 1987, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[8]  M. Alpern,et al.  Variability of accommodation during steady fixation at various levels of illuminance. , 1958, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[9]  F. Toates,et al.  Accommodation function of the human eye. , 1972, Physiological reviews.

[10]  M. Mon-Williams,et al.  Synaesthesia in the normal limb , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[11]  Myron W. Krueger,et al.  Dynamic focusing in head-mounted displays , 1999, Electronic Imaging.

[12]  S. McKee,et al.  A contrast ratio constraint on stereo matching , 1995, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  Martin S. Banks,et al.  A stereo display prototype with multiple focal distances , 2004, SIGGRAPH 2004.

[14]  Thomas Riedl,et al.  Transparent OLED Displays , 2010 .

[15]  Fergal Shevlin A fixed-viewpoint volumetric stereoscopic 3D display using adaptive optics , 2005, IS&T/SPIE Electronic Imaging.

[16]  P. A. Ward,et al.  Modulation dependence of the accommodation response to sinusoidal gratings , 1986, Vision Research.

[17]  David M. Hoffman,et al.  Vergence-accommodation conflicts hinder visual performance and cause visual fatigue. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[18]  M. Ernst,et al.  Focus cues affect perceived depth. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[19]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[20]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[21]  Martin S. Banks,et al.  Creating effective focus cues in multi-plane 3D displays , 2011, Optics express.

[22]  Xu Cheng,et al.  Estimating visual quality from wavefront aberration measurements. , 2003, Journal of refractive surgery.

[23]  Peter A. Howarth,et al.  Visual fatigue caused by viewing stereoscopic motion images: Background, theories, and observations , 2008, Displays.

[24]  F. Okano,et al.  Repeated vergence adaptation causes the decline of visual functions in watching stereoscopic television , 2005, Journal of Display Technology.

[25]  James Gao,et al.  High-speed switchable lens enables the development of a volumetric stereoscopic display. , 2009, Optics express.

[26]  E F FINCHAM,et al.  The reciprocal actions of accommodation and convergence , 1957, The Journal of physiology.

[27]  Kevin J. MacKenzie,et al.  Accommodation to multiple-focal-plane displays: Implications for improving stereoscopic displays and for accommodation control. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[28]  J. C. Kotulak,et al.  A computational model of the error detector of human visual accommodation , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[29]  Campbell Fw A method for measuring the depth of field of the human eye. , 1954 .

[30]  K Simons Effects on stereopsis of monocular versus binocular degradation of image contrast. , 1984, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[31]  D. Fender,et al.  CONTROL MECHANISMS OF THE EYE. , 1964, Scientific American.

[32]  G. Heath,et al.  Components of accommodation. , 1956, American journal of optometry and archives of American Academy of Optometry.

[33]  Arthur Bradley,et al.  Readily visible changes in color contrast are insufficient to stimulate accommodation , 1990, Vision Research.

[34]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  Stereopsis and contrast , 1989, Vision Research.

[35]  Sheng Liu,et al.  An optical see-through head mounted display with addressable focal planes , 2008, 2008 7th IEEE/ACM International Symposium on Mixed and Augmented Reality.

[36]  J. J. KULIKOWSKI,et al.  Limit of single vision in stereopsis depends on contour sharpness , 1978, Nature.

[37]  J. E. Raymond,et al.  The effect of contrast on sustained detection , 1984, Vision Research.

[38]  Philip B. Kruger,et al.  Spatiotemporal transfer function of human accommodation , 1994, Vision Research.

[39]  Martin S. Banks,et al.  Are corresponding points fixed? , 2001, Vision Research.

[40]  Kevin J. MacKenzie,et al.  Vergence and accommodation to multiple-image-plane stereoscopic displays: "real world" responses with practical image-plane separations? , 2012, J. Electronic Imaging.