When thinking rationally increases biases: The role of rational thinking style in escalation of commitment

This paper reports three studies examining the relationship between the rational thinking style (RTS, i.e. individual differences in relying on a conscious, analytical, and relatively affect-free information processing system) and decision quality in an escalation situation. In contrast to conventional wisdom that rational thinking increases decision quality, but consistent with the predictions derived from the cognitive dissonance theory, results of Studies 1 and 2 revealed that high scorers in Pacini and Epstein's (1999) rationality inventory were more likely to have escalation bias than were low scorers. The results further showed that only the ability component of rationality, not the engagement component, was positively correlated with escalating commitment. Similar patterns of results were obtained for situations when participants were personally responsible for prior decisions (Study 1) and when they were not (Study 2). Results of Study 3 showed that the underlying process responsible for the effect of RTS on the escalating tendency is that RTS increases beliefs in prior decisions, which in turn increases escalation. Implications for our understanding of escalation of commitment, rationality theory, and managerial practices are discussed. Cet article rend compte de 3 etudes examinant la relation entre le style de pensee rationnelle (RTS i.e. les differences individuelles reposent sur un systeme de traitement de l’information consciente, analytique et relativement libre d’affects) et la qualite de la decision dans une situation d’escalade d’engagement. Contrairement a l’opinion communement admise pour laquelle la pensee rationnelle accroit la qualite de la decision, mais consistante avec les predictions decoulant de la theorie de la dissonance cognitive, les resultats des etudes 1 et 2 indiquent que des personnes obtenant un score elevea l’Inventaire Rationnel-Experientiel de Pacini et Esptein (1999) sont plus enclines a tomber dans le piege de l’escalade d’engagement que celles dont le score est bas. D’autres resultats montrent que la capacitea la rationalite, non celle a l’engagement, est positivement correlee avec l’escalade d’engagement. Des resultats semblables sont obtenus lors de situations ou les participants sont personnellement responsables de leurs decisions anterieures (Etude 1) ou non (Etude 2). Les resultats de l’etude 3 montrent que le processus responsable de l’effet du RTS sur la tendance a s’engager est le suivant: il accroit les croyances dans les decisions anterieures qui augmentent a leur tour l’engagement. Ces resultats nous permettent de mieux comprendre l’escalade d’engagement, la theorie rationnelle et les pratiques manageriales.

[1]  P. Slovic Risk-taking in children: Age and sex differences. , 1966 .

[2]  Elliot Aronson,et al.  The Theory of Cognitive Dissonance: A Current Perspective1 , 1969 .

[3]  V. Vroom,et al.  Relationship between age and risk taking among managers. , 1971 .

[4]  Barry M. Staw,et al.  Knee-deep in the Big Muddy: A study of escalating commitment to a chosen course of action. , 1976 .

[5]  Barry M. Staw,et al.  Escalation: The Determinants of Commitment to a Chosen Course of Action , 1977 .

[6]  M. Ross,et al.  Facilitation of ego-biased attributions by means of self-serving observer feedback. , 1977 .

[7]  A. Bandura Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. , 1977, Psychological review.

[8]  Barry M. Staw,et al.  Commitment to a Policy Decision: A Multi-Theoretical Perspective. , 1978 .

[9]  Joel Brockner,et al.  Factors affecting withdrawal from an escalating conflict: Quitting before it's too late , 1979 .

[10]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .

[11]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect Theory : An Analysis of Decision under Risk Author ( s ) : , 2007 .

[12]  Barry M. Staw,et al.  The Trapped Administrator: Effects of Job Insecurity and Policy Resistance upon Commitment to a Course of Action. , 1979 .

[13]  B. M. Staw The Escalation of Commitment To a Course of Action , 1981 .

[14]  A. Tversky,et al.  The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. , 1981, Science.

[15]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  The need for cognition. , 1982 .

[16]  Mary Lloyd,et al.  Toward the Reduction of Entrapment , 1982 .

[17]  Claude M. Steele,et al.  DISSONANCE PROCESSES AS SELF-AFFIRMATION , 1983 .

[18]  J. Cooper,et al.  A New Look at Dissonance Theory , 1984 .

[19]  M. Bazerman,et al.  Escalation of commitment in individual and group decision making , 1984 .

[20]  H. Arkes,et al.  The Psychology of Sunk Cost , 1985 .

[21]  Glen Whyte,et al.  Escalating Commitment to a Course of Action: A Reinterpretation , 1986 .

[22]  Bruce E. McCain Continuing Investment Under Conditions of Failure: A Laboratory Study of the Limits to Escalation , 1986 .

[23]  P. Knight,et al.  Humility revisited: Self-esteem, information search, and policy consistency , 1986 .

[24]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  Joel Brockner,et al.  Escalation of Commitment to an Ineffective Course of Action: The Effect of Feedback Having Negative Implications for Self-Identity. , 1986 .

[26]  Edward J. Conlon,et al.  Information requests in the context of escalation. , 1987 .

[27]  I. Simonson,et al.  Choice Based on Reasons: The Case of Attraction and Compromise Effects , 1989 .

[28]  S. Epstein,et al.  Constructive thinking: a broad coping variable with specific components. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  P. Tetlock,et al.  Accountability: a social magnifier of the dilution effect. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  H. Garland Throwing Good Money After Bad: The Effect of Sunk Costs on the Decision to Escalate Commitment to an Ongoing Project , 1990 .

[31]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Thinking too much: introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[32]  S. Epstein,et al.  Unforgettable Experiences: The Relation of Life Events to Basic Beliefs About Self and World , 1992 .

[33]  Itamar Simonson,et al.  Deescalation Strategies: A Comparison of Techniques for Reducing Commitment to Losing Courses of Action , 1992 .

[34]  L. Kirkpatrick,et al.  Cognitive-experiential self-theory and subjective probability: further evidence for two conceptual systems. , 1992 .

[35]  J. Brockner The Escalation of Commitment to a Failing Course of Action: Toward Theoretical Progress , 1992 .

[36]  Donald E. Conlon,et al.  The Role of Project Completion Information in Resource Allocation Decisions , 1993 .

[37]  J. Schaubroeck,et al.  Type A behavior pattern and escalating commitment. , 1993, The Journal of applied psychology.

[38]  S. Epstein Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. , 1994, The American psychologist.

[39]  S. Epstein,et al.  Conflict Between Intuitive and Rational Processing: When People Behave Against Their Better Judgment , 1994 .

[40]  Distinguishing accurate from inaccurate eyewitness identifications via inquiries about decision processes , 1994 .

[41]  Keith D. Markman,et al.  Multiple explanation: A consider-an-alternative strategy for debiasing judgments. , 1995 .

[42]  Irwin P. Levin,et al.  Need for Cognition and Choice Framing Effects , 1996 .

[43]  S. Epstein,et al.  Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[44]  Patricia J. Holahan,et al.  Psychological antecedents of escalation behavior: effects of choice, responsibility, and decision consequences. , 1996, The Journal of applied psychology.

[45]  Glen Whyte,et al.  When success breeds failure: the role of self-efficacy in escalating commitment to a losing course of action , 1997 .

[46]  David J. Sharp,et al.  Project Escalation and Sunk Costs: A test of the International Generalizability of Agency and Prospect Theories , 1997 .

[47]  M. Ashton Personality and job performance: the importance of narrow traits , 1998 .

[48]  Donald E. Conlon,et al.  Too Close to Quit: The Role of Project Completion in Maintaining Commitment1 , 1998 .

[49]  Chi-yue Chiu,et al.  Culture and the construal of agency : Attribution to individual versus group dispositions , 1999 .

[50]  Greg L. Stewart,et al.  Trait bandwidth and stages of job performance : Assessing differential effects for conscientiousness and its subtraits , 1999 .

[51]  Seymour Epstein,et al.  The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. , 1999 .

[52]  H. Moon,et al.  The two faces of conscientiousness: duty and achievement striving in escalation of commitment dilemmas. , 2001, The Journal of applied psychology.

[53]  Henry Moon,et al.  Looking forward and looking back: integrating completion and sunk-cost effects within an escalation-of-commitment progress decision. , 2001, The Journal of applied psychology.

[54]  B. Meglino,et al.  Considering rational self-interest as a disposition: organizational implications of other orientation. , 2004, The Journal of applied psychology.

[55]  Kin Fai Ellick Wong The role of risk in making decisions under escalation situations , 2005 .

[56]  Kin Fai Ellick Wong,et al.  Understanding the emotional aspects of escalation of commitment: the role of negative affect. , 2006, The Journal of applied psychology.

[57]  Kin Fai Ellick Wong,et al.  The role of anticipated regret in escalation of commitment. , 2007, The Journal of applied psychology.