Adaptation to novel visuo-motor transformations: further evidence of functional haptic neglect

Studies with perturbed and augmented haptic information during adaptation to novel visuo-motor transformations suggest that haptic information might be neglected. However, the notion of functional haptic neglect during adaptation is counterintuitive for different reasons. Therefore, we sought conditions where haptic guidance during adaptation results in benefits. First, we mixed guided and unguided trials during practice with a visuo-motor rotation. In two experiments, we found no indication of an advantage of the guidance groups over the no-guidance control groups. In the third experiment, the visuo-motor transformation was a pure time delay because motor timing has been hypothesized to profit from haptic guidance. Again, there was no evidence of benefits for the guidance group. Thus, the findings of this series of experiments provide further evidence of the claim that haptic information is neglected during adaptation to a visuo-motor transformation.

[1]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Two coupled oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping by both hands , 1980, Biological Cybernetics.

[2]  Mathias Hegele,et al.  Adaptation to visuomotor rotations in younger and older adults. , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[3]  N Teasdale,et al.  Mirror drawing in a deafferented patient and normal subjects , 1992, Neurology.

[4]  H. Cunningham Aiming error under transformed spatial mappings suggests a structure for visual-motor maps. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Components of sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly subjects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  Renaud Ronsse,et al.  Multisensory Integration in Dynamical Behaviors: Maximum Likelihood Estimation across Bimanual Skill Learning , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Impossible is nothing: 5:3 and 4:3 multi-frequency bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Flexible Cognitive Strategies during Motor Learning , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[11]  J. Rieser,et al.  Bayesian integration of spatial information. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  The influence of haptic guidance on the production of spatio-temporal patterns. , 2012, Human movement science.

[13]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Transfer of sensorimotor adaptation between different movement categories , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Albert Bandura,et al.  Role of Symbolic Coding and Rehearsal Processes in Observational Learning. , 1973 .

[15]  S. T. Klapp,et al.  Doing two things at once: The role of temporal compatibility , 1979 .

[16]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Proprioception plays a different role for sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions. , 2011, Human movement science.

[17]  H. Imamizu,et al.  The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: implications from intermanual transfer. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Borís Burle,et al.  Direct evidence for cortical suppression of somatosensory afferents during visuomotor adaptation. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Functional independence of explicit and implicit motor adjustments , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[20]  O. Bock,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input: different mechanisms for small versus large rotations , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  C. Winstein,et al.  Effects of physical guidance and knowledge of results on motor learning: support for the guidance hypothesis. , 1994, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[22]  A. Maravita,et al.  Tools for the body (schema) , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  R. Miall,et al.  Digital Object Identifier (DOI) 10.1007/s002219900286 RESEARCH ARTICLE , 2022 .

[24]  E. Brunswik,et al.  The Conceptual Framework of Psychology , 1954 .

[25]  Mathias Hegele,et al.  Implicit and explicit components of dual adaptation to visuomotor rotations , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[26]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Interlimb coordination: Learning and transfer under different feedback conditions , 1997 .

[27]  Yann Coello,et al.  Frame of reference and adaptation to directional bias in a video-controlled reaching task , 2002, Ergonomics.

[28]  M. Kawato,et al.  Trajectory formation of arm movement by cascade neural network model based on minimum torque-change criterion , 1990, Biological Cybernetics.

[29]  F. Bedford Constraints on perceptual learning: objects and dimensions , 1995, Cognition.

[30]  J. Krakauer,et al.  An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Active error corrections enhance adaptation to a visuo-motor rotation , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  Hiroaki Gomi,et al.  Multiple Motor Learning Strategies in Visuomotor Rotation , 2010, PloS one.

[33]  Martin Burghoff,et al.  Visuo-motor adaptation: evidence for a distributed amplitude control system , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[34]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensorimotor discordance: evidence for direction-independent gain control , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[35]  David J. Reinkensmeyer,et al.  Comparison of error-amplification and haptic-guidance training techniques for learning of a timing-based motor task by healthy individuals , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Daniela Balslev,et al.  Enhanced Accuracy in Novel Mirror Drawing after Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation-Induced Proprioceptive Deafferentation , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Tool Use in Action: The Mastery of Complex Visuomotor Transformations , 2013 .

[38]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[39]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Bimanual 1:1 with 90° continuous relative phase: difficult or easy! , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Amplitude differences, spatial assimilation, and integrated feedback in bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  Bill Jones,et al.  Outflow and inflow in movement duplication , 1972 .

[42]  Mathias Hegele,et al.  Generalization of implicit and explicit adjustments to visuomotor rotations across the workspace in younger and older adults. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  Mathias Hegele,et al.  Adaptation to direction-dependent visuo-motor rotations and its decay in younger and older adults. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[44]  Alfred H. Holway,et al.  Determinants of Apparent Visual Size with Distance Variant , 1941 .

[45]  R. Ivry,et al.  An Explicit Strategy Prevails When the Cerebellum Fails to Compute Movement Errors , 2010, The Cerebellum.

[46]  W. Prinz,et al.  Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination , 2001, Nature.

[47]  J. Müsseler,et al.  Perceiving one’s own movements when using a tool , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[48]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[49]  H. Heuer,et al.  Implicit and explicit adjustments to extrinsic visuo-motor transformations and their age-related changes. , 2011, Human movement science.

[50]  David J. Reinkensmeyer,et al.  Haptic Guidance Can Enhance Motor Learning of a Steering Task , 2008, Journal of motor behavior.

[51]  O. Bock,et al.  Mechanisms for sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  M. Ernst,et al.  Optimal integration of shape information from vision and touch , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  The contribution of proprioceptive feedback to sensorimotor adaptation , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  Charles H Shea,et al.  Perceptual and attentional influences on continuous 2:1 and 3:2 multi-frequency bimanual coordination. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  J. Patton,et al.  Can Robots Help the Learning of Skilled Actions? , 2009, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[56]  J. Gordon,et al.  Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  Frans C. T. van der Helm,et al.  Influence of haptic guidance in learning a novel visuomotor task , 2009, Journal of Physiology-Paris.