Adaptation to novel visuo-motor transformations: further evidence of functional haptic neglect
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Mitsuo Kawato,et al. Two coupled oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping by both hands , 1980, Biological Cybernetics.
[2] Mathias Hegele,et al. Adaptation to visuomotor rotations in younger and older adults. , 2008, Psychology and aging.
[3] N Teasdale,et al. Mirror drawing in a deafferented patient and normal subjects , 1992, Neurology.
[4] H. Cunningham. Aiming error under transformed spatial mappings suggests a structure for visual-motor maps. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[5] Otmar Bock,et al. Components of sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly subjects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[6] G. Rizzolatti,et al. The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.
[7] Renaud Ronsse,et al. Multisensory Integration in Dynamical Behaviors: Maximum Likelihood Estimation across Bimanual Skill Learning , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[8] C Ghez,et al. Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[9] Charles H. Shea,et al. Impossible is nothing: 5:3 and 4:3 multi-frequency bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[10] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Flexible Cognitive Strategies during Motor Learning , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[11] J. Rieser,et al. Bayesian integration of spatial information. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.
[12] Herbert Heuer,et al. The influence of haptic guidance on the production of spatio-temporal patterns. , 2012, Human movement science.
[13] Otmar Bock,et al. Transfer of sensorimotor adaptation between different movement categories , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[14] Albert Bandura,et al. Role of Symbolic Coding and Rehearsal Processes in Observational Learning. , 1973 .
[15] S. T. Klapp,et al. Doing two things at once: The role of temporal compatibility , 1979 .
[16] Otmar Bock,et al. Proprioception plays a different role for sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions. , 2011, Human movement science.
[17] H. Imamizu,et al. The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: implications from intermanual transfer. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] Borís Burle,et al. Direct evidence for cortical suppression of somatosensory afferents during visuomotor adaptation. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.
[19] Herbert Heuer,et al. Functional independence of explicit and implicit motor adjustments , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.
[20] O. Bock,et al. Sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input: different mechanisms for small versus large rotations , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[21] C. Winstein,et al. Effects of physical guidance and knowledge of results on motor learning: support for the guidance hypothesis. , 1994, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.
[22] A. Maravita,et al. Tools for the body (schema) , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[23] R. Miall,et al. Digital Object Identifier (DOI) 10.1007/s002219900286 RESEARCH ARTICLE , 2022 .
[24] E. Brunswik,et al. The Conceptual Framework of Psychology , 1954 .
[25] Mathias Hegele,et al. Implicit and explicit components of dual adaptation to visuomotor rotations , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.
[26] S. Swinnen,et al. Interlimb coordination: Learning and transfer under different feedback conditions , 1997 .
[27] Yann Coello,et al. Frame of reference and adaptation to directional bias in a video-controlled reaching task , 2002, Ergonomics.
[28] M. Kawato,et al. Trajectory formation of arm movement by cascade neural network model based on minimum torque-change criterion , 1990, Biological Cybernetics.
[29] F. Bedford. Constraints on perceptual learning: objects and dimensions , 1995, Cognition.
[30] J. Krakauer,et al. An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[31] Herbert Heuer,et al. Active error corrections enhance adaptation to a visuo-motor rotation , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[32] Hiroaki Gomi,et al. Multiple Motor Learning Strategies in Visuomotor Rotation , 2010, PloS one.
[33] Martin Burghoff,et al. Visuo-motor adaptation: evidence for a distributed amplitude control system , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.
[34] Otmar Bock,et al. Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensorimotor discordance: evidence for direction-independent gain control , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.
[35] David J. Reinkensmeyer,et al. Comparison of error-amplification and haptic-guidance training techniques for learning of a timing-based motor task by healthy individuals , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[36] Daniela Balslev,et al. Enhanced Accuracy in Novel Mirror Drawing after Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation-Induced Proprioceptive Deafferentation , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[37] Herbert Heuer,et al. Tool Use in Action: The Mastery of Complex Visuomotor Transformations , 2013 .
[38] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[39] Charles H. Shea,et al. Bimanual 1:1 with 90° continuous relative phase: difficult or easy! , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[40] Charles H. Shea,et al. Amplitude differences, spatial assimilation, and integrated feedback in bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[41] Bill Jones,et al. Outflow and inflow in movement duplication , 1972 .
[42] Mathias Hegele,et al. Generalization of implicit and explicit adjustments to visuomotor rotations across the workspace in younger and older adults. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.
[43] Mathias Hegele,et al. Adaptation to direction-dependent visuo-motor rotations and its decay in younger and older adults. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[44] Alfred H. Holway,et al. Determinants of Apparent Visual Size with Distance Variant , 1941 .
[45] R. Ivry,et al. An Explicit Strategy Prevails When the Cerebellum Fails to Compute Movement Errors , 2010, The Cerebellum.
[46] W. Prinz,et al. Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination , 2001, Nature.
[47] J. Müsseler,et al. Perceiving one’s own movements when using a tool , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.
[48] J. Krakauer,et al. Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.
[49] H. Heuer,et al. Implicit and explicit adjustments to extrinsic visuo-motor transformations and their age-related changes. , 2011, Human movement science.
[50] David J. Reinkensmeyer,et al. Haptic Guidance Can Enhance Motor Learning of a Steering Task , 2008, Journal of motor behavior.
[51] O. Bock,et al. Mechanisms for sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[52] M. Ernst,et al. Optimal integration of shape information from vision and touch , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[53] Jean-Louis Vercher,et al. The contribution of proprioceptive feedback to sensorimotor adaptation , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[54] Charles H Shea,et al. Perceptual and attentional influences on continuous 2:1 and 3:2 multi-frequency bimanual coordination. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[55] J. Patton,et al. Can Robots Help the Learning of Skilled Actions? , 2009, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.
[56] J. Gordon,et al. Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[57] Frans C. T. van der Helm,et al. Influence of haptic guidance in learning a novel visuomotor task , 2009, Journal of Physiology-Paris.