Eye Position Error Influence over “Open-Loop” Smooth Pursuit Initiation

The oculomotor system integrates a variety of visual signals into appropriate motor plans, but such integration can have widely varying time scales. For example, smooth pursuit eye movements to follow a moving target are slower and longer lasting than saccadic eye movements and it has been suggested that initiating a smooth pursuit eye movement involves an obligatory “open-loop” interval in which new visual motion signals presumably cannot influence the ensuing motor plan for up to 100 ms after movement initiation. However, this view is contrary to the idea that the oculomotor periphery has privileged access to short-latency visual signals. Here, we show that smooth pursuit initiation is sensitive to visual inputs, even in open-loop intervals. We instructed male rhesus macaque monkeys to initiate saccade-free smooth pursuit eye movements and injected a transient, instantaneous eye position error signal at different times relative to movement initiation. We found robust short-latency modulations in eye velocity and acceleration, starting only ∼50 ms after transient signal occurrence and even during open-loop pursuit initiation. Critically, the spatial direction of the injected position error signal had predictable effects on smooth pursuit initiation, with forward errors increasing eye acceleration and backward errors reducing it. Catch-up saccade frequencies and amplitudes were also similarly altered ∼50 ms after transient signals, much like the well known effects on microsaccades during fixation. Our results demonstrate that smooth pursuit initiation is highly sensitive to visual signals and that catch-up saccade generation is reset after a visual transient. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Smooth pursuit eye movements allow us to track moving objects. The first ∼100 ms of smooth pursuit initiation are characterized by smooth eye acceleration and are overwhelmingly described as being “open-loop”; that is, unmodifiable by new visual motion signals. We found that all phases of smooth pursuit, including the so-called open-loop intervals, are reliably modifiable by visual signals. We injected transient flashes resulting in very brief, spatially specific position error signals to smooth pursuit and observed very short-latency changes in smooth eye movements to minimize such errors. Our results highlight the flexibility of the oculomotor system in reacting to environmental events and suggest a functional role for the pervasiveness of visual sensitivity in oculomotor control brain regions.

[1]  C Busettini,et al.  Pontine omnipause activity during conjugate and disconjugate eye movements in macaques. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  E. Reingold,et al.  Saccadic Inhibition and Gaze Contingent Research Paradigms , 2000 .

[3]  Ziad M Hafed,et al.  Dynamics of fixational eye position and microsaccades during spatial cueing: the case of express microsaccades. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Brian D Corneil,et al.  Dynamic and Opposing Adjustment of Movement Cancellation and Generation in an Oculomotor Countermanding Task , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Ziad M. Hafed Alteration of Visual Perception prior to Microsaccades , 2013, Neuron.

[6]  Kitty Z. Xu,et al.  Inhibition of voluntary saccadic eye movement commands by abrupt visual onsets. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Karl R. Gegenfurtner,et al.  Contrast sensitivity during the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements , 2007, Vision Research.

[8]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention , 2003, Vision Research.

[9]  Eyal M. Reingold,et al.  SACCADIC INHIBITION IN COMPLEX VISUAL TASKS , 1999 .

[10]  Alain Guillaume,et al.  Saccadic inhibition is accompanied by large and complex amplitude modulations when induced by visual backward masking. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[11]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  Inhibition of steady-state smooth pursuit and catch-up saccades by abrupt visual and auditory onsets. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Improved visual sensitivity during smooth pursuit eye movements , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[14]  B. Fischer,et al.  Saccadic eye movements after extremely short reaction times in the monkey , 1983, Brain Research.

[15]  Antimo Buonocore,et al.  Saccadic inhibition underlies the remote distractor effect , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Chih-Yang Chen,et al.  Spatial frequency sensitivity in macaque midbrain , 2018, Nature Communications.

[17]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Modulation of Microsaccades in Monkey during a Covert Visual Attention Task , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  B. Fischer,et al.  Human express saccades: extremely short reaction times of goal directed eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  Etienne Olivier,et al.  Visual Responses on Neck Muscles Reveal Selective Gating that Prevents Express Saccades , 2004, Neuron.

[20]  Stephen J Heinen,et al.  A foveal target increases catch-up saccade frequency during smooth pursuit. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  F A Miles,et al.  Release of fixation for pursuit and saccades in humans: evidence for shared inputs acting on different neural substrates. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  A. Bompas,et al.  Saccadic Inhibition Reveals the Timing of Automatic and Voluntary Signals in the Human Brain , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  D. Munoz,et al.  Overt Responses during Covert Orienting , 2014, Neuron.

[24]  F A Miles,et al.  Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. II. Dependence on a prior saccadic eye movement. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Decreases in the Latency of Smooth Pursuit and Saccadic Eye Movements Produced by the “Gap Paradigm” in the Monkey , 1996, Vision Research.

[26]  D. Munoz,et al.  Comparison of the discharge characteristics of brain stem omnipause neurons and superior colliculus fixation neurons in monkey: implications for control of fixation and saccade behavior. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  B. Fischer,et al.  Express-saccades of the monkey: Reaction times versus intensity, size, duration, and eccentricity of their targets , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  M Missal,et al.  Common inhibitory mechanism for saccades and smooth-pursuit eye movements. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  A Microsaccadic Account of Attentional Capture and Inhibition of Return in Posner Cueing , 2016, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[30]  Ziad M Hafed,et al.  The transfer function of the rhesus macaque oculomotor system for small-amplitude slow motion trajectories , 2018, bioRxiv.

[31]  Harry J. Wyatt,et al.  Target position and velocity: The stimuli for smooth pursuit eye movements , 1980, Vision Research.

[32]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.

[33]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades , 2015, Current Biology.

[34]  K. Cullen,et al.  A non-visual mechanism for voluntary cancellation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  M. Missal,et al.  What triggers catch-up saccades during visual tracking? , 2002, Journal of Neurophysiology.

[36]  A. Fuchs,et al.  A method for measuring horizontal and vertical eye movement chronically in the monkey. , 1966, Journal of applied physiology.

[37]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  A neural locus for spatial-frequency specific saccadic suppression in visual-motor neurons of the primate superior colliculus. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  L. Minor,et al.  Horizontal vestibuloocular reflex evoked by high-acceleration rotations in the squirrel monkey. II. Responses after canal plugging. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  S G Lisberger,et al.  The latency of pathways containing the site of motor learning in the monkey vestibulo-ocular reflex. , 1984, Science.

[40]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Visual motion processing for the initiation of smooth-pursuit eye movements in humans. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  Ziad M. Hafed Mechanisms for generating and compensating for the smallest possible saccades , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[42]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Direct evidence for a position input to the smooth pursuit system. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  E. Keller Participation of medial pontine reticular formation in eye movement generation in monkey. , 1974, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Activity of omnipause neurons in alert cats during saccadic eye movements and visual stimuli. , 1982, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  E Kowler,et al.  Smooth pursuit of small-amplitude sinusoidal motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[46]  Eyal M. Reingold,et al.  Saccadic Inhibition in Voluntary and Reflexive Saccades , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  David Melcher,et al.  Beyond the point of no return: effects of visual distractors on saccade amplitude and velocity. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  L. Snyder,et al.  Effect of viewing distance and location of the axis of head rotation on the monkey's vestibuloocular reflex. I. Eye movement responses. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  Chih-Yang Chen,et al.  Alteration of the microsaccadic velocity-amplitude main sequence relationship after visual transients: implications for models of saccade control , 2017 .

[50]  R. Krauzlis Recasting the smooth pursuit eye movement system. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  C. Rashbass,et al.  The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[52]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[53]  L. Minor,et al.  Horizontal vestibuloocular reflex evoked by high-acceleration rotations in the squirrel monkey. I. Normal responses. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  Stephen J Heinen,et al.  Choosing a foveal goal recruits the saccadic system during smooth pursuit. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  Erik Blaser,et al.  The accuracy and precision of saccades to small and large targets , 1995, Vision Research.

[56]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Microsaccadic Suppression of Visual Bursts in the Primate Superior Colliculus , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  Alexander C. Schütz,et al.  Visual sensitivity for luminance and chromatic stimuli during the execution of smooth pursuit and saccadic eye movements , 2017, Vision Research.

[58]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Distractor interference during smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[59]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  On the Dissociation between Microsaccade Rate and Direction after Peripheral Cues: Microsaccadic Inhibition Revisited , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurons with complex visual properties in the superior colliculus of the macaque monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[61]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Vision, Perception, and Attention through the Lens of Microsaccades: Mechanisms and Implications , 2015, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[62]  Antimo Buonocore,et al.  Saccade Reorienting Is Facilitated by Pausing the Oculomotor Program , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Toward a model of microsaccade generation: the case of microsaccadic inhibition. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[64]  B. Richmond,et al.  Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: An improved method , 1980, Vision Research.

[65]  A. Leventhal,et al.  Signal timing across the macaque visual system. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[66]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[67]  D. Munoz,et al.  On the importance of the transient visual response in the superior colliculus , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[68]  W. Bialek,et al.  The Neural Basis for Combinatorial Coding in a Cortical Population Response , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  James J. Clark,et al.  Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts , 2002, Vision Research.

[70]  You Yun Lee,et al.  Quantitative assessment of divergence eye movements. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[71]  Chih-Yang Chen,et al.  Postmicrosaccadic Enhancement of Slow Eye Movements , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[72]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Different responses to small visual errors during initiation and maintenance of smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[73]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  A Neural Mechanism for Microsaccade Generation in the Primate Superior Colliculus , 2009, Science.