Explaining school mathematics performance from symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude processing: similarities and differences between typical and low-achieving children

Magnitude processing is one of the most central cognitive mechanisms that underlie persistent mathematics difficulties. No consensus has yet been reached about whether these difficulties can be predominantly attributed to deficits in symbolic or nonsymbolic magnitude processing. To investigate this issue, we assessed symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude representations in children with low or typical achievement in school mathematics. Response latencies and the distance effect were comparable between groups in both symbolic and nonsymbolic tasks. The results indicated that both typical and low achievers were able to access magnitude representation via symbolic and nonsymbolic processing. However, low achievers presented higher error rates than typical achievers, especially in the nonsymbolic task. Furthermore, measures of nonsymbolic magnitude explained individual differences in school mathematics better than measures of symbolic magnitude when considering all of the children together. When examining the groups separately, symbolic magnitude representation explained differences in school mathematics in low achievers but not in typical achievers. These results suggest that symbolic magnitude is more relevant to solving arithmetic problems when mathematics achievement is particularly low. In contrast, individual differences in nonsymbolic processing appear to be related to mathematics achievement in a more general manner.

[1]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: a study of 8–9-year-old students , 2004, Cognition.

[2]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  A longitudinal study of mathematical competencies in children with specific mathematics difficulties versus children with comorbid mathematics and reading difficulties. , 2003, Child development.

[3]  Mary K. Hoard,et al.  Learning Disabilities in Arithmetic and Mathematics Theoretical and Empirical Perspectives , 2004 .

[4]  Klaus Willmes,et al.  Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins , 2001, Cognition.

[5]  A. Roazzi,et al.  T.D.E: Teste do desempenho escolar: manual para aplicação e interpretação , 2004 .

[6]  Bert De Smedt,et al.  Defective number module or impaired access? Numerical magnitude processing in first graders with mathematical difficulties. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[7]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Core information processing deficits in developmental dyscalculia and low numeracy. , 2008, Developmental science.

[8]  D. Bandeira,et al.  Matrizes progressivas coloridas de Raven - escala especial: normas para Porto Alegre, RS , 2004 .

[9]  Andrea Facoetti,et al.  Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[10]  M. Brysbaert Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. , 1995 .

[11]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[12]  M. Noël,et al.  Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing , 2007, Cognition.

[13]  M. Mazzocco,et al.  Cognitive Characteristics of Children With Mathematics Learning Disability (MLD) Vary as a Function of the Cutoff Criterion Used to Define MLD , 2007, Journal of learning disabilities.

[14]  G. Mandler,et al.  Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  S. Dehaene Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.

[16]  E. Walker,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 2013 .

[17]  Karin Landerl,et al.  Typical and atypical development of basic numerical skills in elementary school. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[18]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[19]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Symbolic and nonsymbolic number comparison in children with and without dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[20]  R. Shalev,et al.  Developmental Dyscalculia , 2004, Journal of child neurology.

[21]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalculia). , 2011, Child development.

[22]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cerebral Pathways for Calculation: Double Dissociation between Rote Verbal and Quantitative Knowledge of Arithmetic , 1997, Cortex.

[23]  S. Varma,et al.  Dyscalculia: From Brain to Education , 2011, Science.

[24]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[25]  Kristina Moll,et al.  Dyslexia and dyscalculia: two learning disorders with different cognitive profiles. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.