Children’s mappings between number words and the approximate number system

Humans can represent number either exactly--using their knowledge of exact numbers as supported by language, or approximately--using their approximate number system (ANS). Adults can map between these two systems--they can both translate from an approximate sense of the number of items in a brief visual display to a discrete number word estimate (i.e., ANS-to-Word), and can generate an approximation, for example by rapidly tapping, when provided with an exact verbal number (i.e., Word-to-ANS). Here we ask how these mappings are initially formed and whether one mapping direction may become functional before the other during development. In two experiments, we gave 2-5 year old children both an ANS-to-Word task, where they had to give a verbal number response to an approximate presentation (i.e., after seeing rapidly flashed dots, or watching rapid hand taps), and a Word-to-ANS task, where they had to generate an approximate response to a verbal number request (i.e., rapidly tapping after hearing a number word). Replicating previous results, children did not successfully generate numerically appropriate verbal responses in the ANS-to-Word task until after 4 years of age--well after they had acquired the Cardinality Principle of verbal counting. In contrast, children successfully generated numerically appropriate tapping sequences in the Word-to-ANS task before 4 years of age--well before many understood the Cardinality Principle. We further found that the accuracy of the mapping between the ANS and number words, as captured by error rates, continues to develop after this initial formation of the interface. These results suggest that the mapping between the ANS and verbal number representations is not functionally bidirectional in early development, and that the mapping direction from number representations to the ANS is established before the reverse.

[1]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[2]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[3]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children , 2006, Cognition.

[4]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  A Rational-Constructivist Account of Early Learning About Numbers and Objects , 1993 .

[5]  Lisa Feigenson,et al.  The equality of quantity , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Elena Nicoladis,et al.  Are number gestures easier than number words for preschoolers , 2010 .

[7]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  Comprehension, Production, and Language Acquisition , 1983 .

[8]  Silke M. Göbel,et al.  Impact of High Mathematics Education on the Number Sense , 2012, PloS one.

[9]  S. Carey,et al.  Re-visiting the competence/performance debate in the acquisition of the counting principles , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  C. Gilmore,et al.  Children's mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic representations of number. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[11]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  The development of language and abstract concepts: the case of natural number. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[12]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Does the Parietal Cortex Distinguish between “10,” “Ten,” and Ten Dots? , 2007, Neuron.

[13]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[14]  R. Church,et al.  A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[15]  S. Carey,et al.  How counting represents number: What children must learn and when they learn it , 2008, Cognition.

[16]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Comprar Space, Time And Number In The Brain, Searching For The Foundations Of Mathematical Thought | S. Dehaene | 9780123859488 | Academic Press , 2011 .

[17]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Measurement Estimation: Learning to Map the Route From Number to Quantity and Back , 1998 .

[18]  Clarissa A. Thompson,et al.  Early development of spatial-numeric associations: evidence from spatial and quantitative performance of preschoolers. , 2010, Developmental science.

[19]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Preschool children's mapping of number words to nonsymbolic numerosities. , 2005, Child development.

[20]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The Child's Understanding of Number , 1979 .

[21]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  Sara Cordes,et al.  The development of area discrimination and its implications for number representation in infancy. , 2006, Developmental science.

[23]  S. Schiffer Meanings and Concepts , 1999 .

[24]  M. Ebersbach,et al.  Symbolic versus non-symbolic magnitude estimations among children and adults. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[25]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Calibrating the mental number line , 2008, Cognition.

[26]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[27]  A. Senghas,et al.  Children Creating Core Properties of Language: Evidence from an Emerging Sign Language in Nicaragua , 2004, Science.

[28]  D. Scott,et al.  The development of language. , 1974, Queen's nursing journal.

[29]  Wim Fias,et al.  Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Pathways of Number Processing , 2008 .

[30]  J. Enns,et al.  Life Span Changes in Visual Enumeration: The Number Discrimination Task , 1996 .

[31]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cerebral Pathways for Calculation: Double Dissociation between Rote Verbal and Quantitative Knowledge of Arithmetic , 1997, Cortex.

[32]  Xavier Seron,et al.  Numerical estimation in blind subjects: evidence of the impact of blindness and its following experience. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.

[34]  B. Landau,et al.  Whence and whither in spatial language and spatial cognition , 1993 .

[35]  Xavier Seron,et al.  Over-estimation in numerosity estimation tasks: more than an attentional bias? , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[36]  Christine Schiltz,et al.  Estimation abilities of large numerosities in Kindergartners , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[37]  Susan Carey,et al.  One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles , 2007, Cognition.

[38]  S. Carey The Origin of Concepts , 2000 .

[39]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments , 2004, Neuron.

[40]  O. Svenson,et al.  Speeds of Subitizing and Counting Processes in Different Age Groups , 1983 .

[41]  Charles Francis Hockett,et al.  Refurbishing our Foundations: Elementary linguistics from an advanced point of view , 1987 .

[42]  C. R. Gallistel,et al.  Commentary on Le Corre & Carey , 2007, Cognition.

[43]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[44]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[45]  Sotaro Kita,et al.  Response to Comment on "Children Creating Core Properties of Language: Evidence from an Emerging Sign Language in Nicaragua" , 2005, Science.

[46]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Beyond the number domain , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  L. Feigenson,et al.  Multiple Spatially Overlapping Sets Can Be Enumerated in Parallel , 2006, Psychological science.

[48]  Tim Hunter,et al.  Young children's understanding of "more" and discrimination of number and surface area. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[49]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[50]  Xavier Seron,et al.  Under- and over-estimation: a bi-directional mapping process between symbolic and non-symbolic representations of number? , 2011, Experimental psychology.

[51]  Barbara Landau,et al.  4 - Perceptual Units and Their Mapping with Language , 2001 .

[52]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Developmental change in the acuity of approximate number and area representations. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[53]  K. Wynn Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[54]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Numerical Estimation in Adults with and without Developmental Dyscalculia. , 2012 .

[55]  Lisa Feigenson,et al.  A double-dissociation in infants' representations of object arrays , 2005, Cognition.

[56]  K. Fuson Children's Counting and Concepts of Number , 1987 .

[57]  T K LANDAUER,et al.  Rate of Implicit Speech , 1962, Perceptual and motor skills.

[58]  O. Svenson,et al.  Subitizing and counting processes in young children , 1978 .

[59]  K. Davidson,et al.  Does learning to count involve a semantic induction? , 2012, Cognition.

[60]  Justin Halberda,et al.  The Development of “Most” Comprehension and Its Potential Dependence on Counting Ability in Preschoolers , 2008 .

[61]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[62]  S. Dehaene Origins of Mathematical Intuitions , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[63]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Number without a language model , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[64]  Robin S. Chapman,et al.  Who is ‘Daddy’ revisited: the status of two-year-olds' over-extended words in use and comprehension , 1977, Journal of Child Language.

[65]  W. STANLEY JEVONS,et al.  The Power of Numerical Discrimination , 1871, Nature.

[66]  Susan C. Johnson,et al.  An association between understanding cardinality and analog magnitude representations in preschoolers , 2011, Cognition.

[67]  A. Baroody,et al.  The Estimation of Set Size by Potentially Gifted Kindergarten-Age Children , 1991 .

[68]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  A parieto-frontal network for visual numerical information in the monkey. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[69]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Speed discrimination in 6- and 10-month-old infants follows Weber's law. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[70]  V. Walsh,et al.  The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[71]  Sylvie Droit-Volet,et al.  Time, Number and Length: Similarities and Differences in Discrimination in Adults and Children , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[72]  Lester E. Krueger,et al.  Perceived numerosity , 1972 .