Selective use of perceptual recalibration versus visuomotor skill acquisition.

Exposure to laterally displacing prisms is characterized by systematic misreaching in the opposite direction after prisms are removed. Other learning tasks involving altered visuomotor mappings can often be mastered by the subject with minimal resulting aftereffects. One variable that may account for this difference is the nature of the feedback provided to the subject: during studies of prism exposure, subjects usually view the hand itself, whereas in many studies of visuomotor learning, subjects view a computer-generated representation of the hand position or movement. We compared the use of actual feedback of the hand with computer-generated representational feedback of its position during exposure to laterally displacing prisms. In the actual feedback condition (ACT), a light on the fingertip was illuminated immediately at the end of each reach. In the representational feedback condition (REP), a computer-generated spot of light was displayed to indicate the exact position of the fingertip at the end of each reach. Whereas the rate and magnitude of error correction were the same in both conditions, only the ACT condition produced the large adaptive aftereffect typically observed after prism exposure. These results suggest that the perception of a physical coincidence between the feedback source and the hand may be a key factor in determining whether adaptation is accomplished through perceptual recalibration or visuomotor skill acquisition.

[1]  J R Flanagan,et al.  Composition and Decomposition of Internal Models in Motor Learning under Altered Kinematic and Dynamic Environments , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  R B Welch,et al.  Variables affecting the intermanual transfer and decay of prism adaptation. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  M. Perenin,et al.  Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect , 1998, Nature.

[4]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  T. Brashers-Krug,et al.  Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  J R Lackner,et al.  Adaptation to Visual Displacement: Contribution of Proprioceptive, Visual, and Attentional Factors , 1981, Perception.

[7]  Tracy L. Faber,et al.  Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching , 1996, Nature.

[8]  S. Wise,et al.  Changes in motor cortical activity during visuomotor adaptation , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Modular decomposition in visuomotor learning , 1997, Nature.

[10]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  Felice L. Bedford,et al.  Perceptual and cognitive spatial learning. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  K. Kurata,et al.  Reacquisition deficits in prism adaptation after muscimol microinjection into the ventral premotor cortex of monkeys. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  J. Lackner,et al.  The role of brachial muscle spindle signals in assignment of visual direction. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Robert B. Welch,et al.  Multiple concurrent visual-motor mappings: implications for models of adaptation. , 1994 .

[15]  Ivan Toni,et al.  Eye position tunes the contribution of allocentric and egocentric information to target localization in human goal-directed arm movements , 1997, Neuroscience Letters.

[16]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Evidence for an eye-centered spherical representation of the visuomotor map. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  M. Hallett,et al.  Adaptation to lateral displacement of vision in patients with lesions of the central nervous system , 1983, Neurology.

[19]  M. Kawato,et al.  Cerebro-cerebellar functional connectivity revealed by the laterality index in tool-use learning. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[20]  Dottie M. Clower,et al.  The Inferior Parietal Lobule Is the Target of Output from the Superior Colliculus, Hippocampus, and Cerebellum , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  R. Held,et al.  Adaptation to displaced and delayed visual feedback from the hand. , 1966 .

[22]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  R B Welch,et al.  Multiple concurrent visual-motor mappings: implications for models of adaptation. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  C. S. Harris Perceptual adaptation to inverted, reversed, and displaced vision. , 1965, Psychological review.

[26]  R. Held,et al.  Adaptation of Disarranged Hand-Eye Coordination Contingent upon Re-Afferent Stimulation , 1958 .

[27]  S. Chieffi,et al.  Visual illusion and action , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  J. Uhlarik,et al.  Role of cognitive factors on adaptation to prismatic displacement. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[29]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  J R Lackner,et al.  Proprioceptive influences on auditory and visual spatial localization , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  Felice L. Bedford,et al.  Keeping perception accurate , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  H. Imamizu,et al.  The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: implications from intermanual transfer. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  B. McGonigle,et al.  Serial Adaptation to Conflicting Prismatic Rearrangement Effects in Monkey and Man , 1977, Perception.

[35]  P. Bertelson,et al.  Adaptation to auditory-visual discordance and ventriloquism in semirealistic situations , 1977 .

[36]  J R Flanagan,et al.  Trajectory adaptation to a nonlinear visuomotor transformation: evidence of motion planning in visually perceived space. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Martin Burghoff,et al.  Visuo-motor adaptation: evidence for a distributed amplitude control system , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[38]  R. Welch The effect of experienced limb identity upon adaptation to simulated displacement of the visual field , 1972 .

[39]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Alternating prism exposure causes dual adaptation and generalization to a novel displacement , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.