The relationship of input and output phonological processing: An evaluation of models and evidence to support them

In this paper, we review studies of the relationship between input and output phonological processing and discuss the means by which an interactive activation model that assumes a single phonological network or functionally connected input and output phonological networks could account for apparent dissociations of these two pathways. Following this, we report data from 24 aphasic subjects with word processing deficits that indicate associations between input and output phonological processing. Input phonological measures correlated with output phonological measures, but not with output lexical-semantic measures. Input lexical-semantic measures did not significantly correlate with any of the output measures. We identified one subject, EF, who did not show this overall pattern. She performed well on two measures of phonological input processing (discrimination and rhyme judgements), but produced a high rate of phonological errors in picture naming. On an auditory lexical decision task, however, EF produced a high rate of false alarm errors (misperception of nonwords as words). False alarm errors have been attributed to a disturbance in input phonological processing. Consistent with this hypothesis, the rates of false alarm errors made by this group of subjects on the same auditory lexical decision task correlated with (1) input tasks that require maintaining activation of phonological representations and (2) a measure of output phonological processing (rates of phonologically related nonword errors in picture naming). These results are discussed with reference to current approaches to the study of input and output phonological processing and possible future investigations of this question.

[1]  R. Burchfield Frequency Analysis of English Usage: Lexicon and Grammar. By W. Nelson Francis and Henry Kučera with the assistance of Andrew W. Mackie. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 1982. x + 561 , 1985 .

[2]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Where Do Semantic Errors Come From? , 1990, Cortex.

[3]  Judith F. Kroll,et al.  Lexical access for concrete and abstract words. , 1986 .

[4]  G. Dell,et al.  Models of Impaired Lexical Access in Speech Production , 2000 .

[5]  Nadine Martin Eleanor M. Saffran Language and Auditory-verbal Short-term Memory Impairments: Evidence for Common Underlying Processes , 1997 .

[6]  Dissociations between component mechanisms in short-term memory: evidence from brain-damaged patients , 1993 .

[7]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The selective impairment of phonological processing: A case study , 1983, Brain and Language.

[8]  W. Levelt,et al.  Monitoring and self-repair in speech , 1983, Cognition.

[9]  T Shallice,et al.  THE SELECTIVE IMPAIRMENT OF THE PHONOLOGICAL OUTPUT BUFFER , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[10]  T. Shallice,et al.  Isolating Cognitive Modules with the Dual-Task Paradigm: Are Speech Perception and Production Separate Processes? , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[11]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming , 1991 .

[12]  G. Dell,et al.  The role of computational models in neuropsychological investigations of language: reply to Ruml and Caramazza (2000). , 2000, Psychological review.

[13]  R. Nass,et al.  The assessment of aphasia and related disorders By Harold Goodglass and edith kaplan philadelphia, lea & febiger, 1983 illustrated, $27.50 (package) , 1984 .

[14]  Gerhard Blanken,et al.  Formal paraphasias: A single case study , 1990, Brain and Language.

[15]  H. Coslett,et al.  Reversal of the concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia , 1994 .

[16]  Markus F. Damian,et al.  The Relation of Phoneme Discrimination, Lexical Access, and Short-Term Memory: A Case Study and Interactive Activation Account , 1999, Brain and Language.

[17]  Tim Shallice,et al.  Case study approach in neuropsychological research , 1979 .

[18]  E K Warrington,et al.  Concrete word dyslexia. , 1981, British journal of psychology.

[19]  S. Blumstein,et al.  Phonological processing and lexical access in aphasia , 1988, Brain and Language.

[20]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  Vocabulary Acquisition and Verbal Short-Term Memory: Computational and Neural Bases , 1997, Brain and Language.

[21]  F. Rose Missing the Meaning? A Cognitive Neuropsychological Study of Processing of Words by an Aphasic Patient , 1989 .

[22]  G. Dell,et al.  Origins of Paraphasias in Deep Dysphasia: Testing the Consequences of a Decay Impairment to an Interactive Spreading Activation Model of Lexical Retrieval , 1994, Brain and Language.

[23]  M. Schlossberg The Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders. 2nd ed. , 1984 .

[24]  The development of disordered phonology: A case study , 1987 .

[25]  Hugh W. Buckingham,et al.  Phonological aspects of aphasia , 1981 .

[26]  David C. Plaut,et al.  The emergence of phonology from the interplay of speech comprehension and production ; A distributed connectionist approach , 1998 .

[27]  D. Allport,et al.  Speech Production and Comprehension: One Lexicon or Two? , 1984 .

[28]  Myrna F. Schwartz,et al.  The Philadelphia Naming Test: Scoring and Rationale , 1996 .

[29]  N. Martin,et al.  A computational account of deep dysphasia: Evidence from a single case study , 1992, Brain and Language.

[30]  G. Dell,et al.  Stages of lexical access in language production , 1992, Cognition.

[31]  D. Howard,et al.  Phonological Errors in Aphasic Naming: Comprehension, Monitoring and Lexicality , 1995, Cortex.

[32]  N. Martin,et al.  How Can Connectionist Cognitive Models of Language Inform Models of Language Rehabilitation , 2002 .

[33]  T. Shallice From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: Converging Operations: Specific Syndromes and Evidence from Normal Subjects , 1988 .

[34]  S. Breedin Reversal of the concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia , 1994 .

[35]  Carlo Caltagirone,et al.  Some aspects of phonological impairment in aphasia , 1980, Brain and Language.

[36]  R. R. Peterson,et al.  Lexical selection and phonological encoding during language production: Evidence for cascaded processing. , 1998 .

[37]  Lyndsey Nickels,et al.  Fractionating the Articulatory Loop: Dissociations and Associations in Phonological Recoding in Aphasia , 1997, Brain and Language.

[38]  G. Dell,et al.  Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. , 1997, Psychological review.

[39]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Independence of Input and Output Phonology in Word Processing and Short-Term Memory , 1999 .

[40]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Language Processing and Working Memory: Neuropsychological Evidence for Separate Phonological and Semantic Capacities , 1994 .

[41]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .

[42]  Cristina Romani,et al.  Are there distinct input and output buffers? Evidence from an aphasic patient with an impaired output buffer , 1992 .

[43]  D Howard,et al.  Frozen phonology thawed: the analysis and remediation of a developmental disorder of real word phonology. , 1992, European journal of disorders of communication : the journal of the College of Speech and Language Therapists, London.

[44]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[45]  W. Nelson Francis,et al.  FREQUENCY ANALYSIS OF ENGLISH USAGE: LEXICON AND GRAMMAR , 1983 .