When does haste make waste? Speed-accuracy tradeoff, skill level, and the tools of the trade.

Novice and skilled golfers took a series of golf putts with a standard putter (Exp. 1) or a distorted funny putter (consisting of an s-shaped and arbitrarily weighted putter shaft; Exp. 2) under instructions to either (a) take as much time as needed to be accurate or to (b) putt as fast as possible while still being accurate. Planning and movement time were measured for each putt. In both experiments, novices produced the typical speed-accuracy trade-off. Going slower, in terms of both the planning and movement components of execution, improved performance. In contrast, skilled golfers benefited from reduced performance time when using the standard putter in Exp. 1 and, specifically, taking less time to plan improved performance. In Exp. 2, skilled golfers improved by going slower when using the funny putter, but only when it was unfamiliar. Thus, skilled performance benefits from speed instructions when wielding highly familiar tools (i.e., the standard putter) is harmed when using new tools (i.e., the funny putter), and benefits again by speed instructions as the new tool becomes familiar. Planning time absorbs these changes.

[1]  S. Iversen Motor control , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[2]  Lowe Bryan William,et al.  Studies on the telegraphic language: The acquisition of a hierarchy of habits. , 1899 .

[3]  G. Stelmach Motor Control: Issues and Trends , 1976 .

[4]  T. Carr,et al.  Automaticity in skill acquisition: Mechanisms for reducing interference in concurrent performance. , 1989 .

[5]  R. Gray,et al.  Effects of focus of attention on baseball batting performance in players of differing skill levels. , 2007, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[6]  C. Hillman,et al.  Cortico-cortical communication and superior performance in skilled marksmen: an EEG coherence analysis. , 2003 .

[7]  Caroline E. Zsambok,et al.  Characteristics of Skilled Option Generation in Chess , 1995 .

[8]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  When High-Powered People Fail , 2005, Psychological science.

[9]  Janine E. Janosky,et al.  Ethics and Single Subject Research , 2009 .

[10]  W. Schneider,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of practice-related change: fMRI and meta-analytic evidence of a domain-general control network for learning. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[11]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[12]  Sher ry Folsom-Meek,et al.  Human Performance , 2020, Nature.

[13]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[14]  Richard E. Lucas,et al.  Cognitive determinants of affective forecasting errors. , 2010, Judgment and decision making.

[15]  R. Masters Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure , 1992 .

[16]  J. Vickers,et al.  Visual control when aiming at a far target. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  Paul J. Feltovich,et al.  Categorization and Representation of Physics Problems by Experts and Novices , 1981, Cogn. Sci..

[18]  Janine E. Janosky,et al.  Application of the Single Subject Design in Biomedicine , 2009 .

[19]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Effects of focus of attention depend on golfers' skill , 2003, Journal of sports sciences.

[20]  J. Maxwell,et al.  The relationship between initial errorless learning conditions and subsequent performance. , 2005, Human movement science.

[21]  Michael Hoerger,et al.  Effects of situational demand upon social enjoyment and preference in schizotypy. , 2007, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[22]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .

[23]  D. BRADLEY,et al.  The Psychophysiology of Sport A Mechanistic Understanding of the Psychology of Superior Performance , 2006 .

[24]  Launi K. Meili,et al.  Expertise differences in cortical activation and gaze behavior during rifle shooting. , 2000 .

[25]  S. Keele,et al.  The Structure of Motor Programs , 1976 .

[26]  P. Dixon,et al.  Semantics affect the planning but not control of grasping , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  A Mark Williams,et al.  Online Attentional-Focus Manipulations in a Soccer-Dribbling Task: Implications for the Proceduralization of Motor Skills , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[29]  Richard E. Lucas,et al.  Immune neglect in affective forecasting. , 2009 .

[30]  Joseph G. Johnson,et al.  Expertise-based differences in search and option-generation strategies. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[31]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Putting in the mind versus putting on the green: Expertise, performance time, and the linking of imagery and action , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[32]  John R. Anderson Acquisition of cognitive skill. , 1982 .

[33]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[34]  R. Proctor,et al.  Skill acquisition and human performance , 1995 .

[35]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Expertise, attention, and memory in sensorimotor skill execution: Impact of novel task constraints on dual-task performance and episodic memory , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[36]  H. Hausenblas,et al.  Handbook of Sport Psychology , 2020 .

[37]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  Cortical activations in primary and secondary motor areas during complex bimanual movements in professional piano players , 2000, NeuroImage.

[38]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .

[39]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[40]  Steven L. Small,et al.  The mind of expert motor performance is cool and focused , 2007, NeuroImage.

[41]  Janine E. Janosky,et al.  Methodological Framework for Single Subject Designs , 2009 .

[42]  S. Petersen,et al.  Practice-related changes in human brain functional anatomy during nonmotor learning. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Rules of the Mind , 1993 .

[44]  P. Ackerman,et al.  Motivation and cognitive abilities: an integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition , 1989 .

[45]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Grasping the meaning of words , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[47]  J. Kruger,et al.  Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[48]  R. Gray Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: expertise differences, choking, and slumps. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[49]  D. G. MacKay The Problems of Flexibility, Fluency, and Speed-Accuracy Trade-Off in Skilled Behavior. , 1982 .

[50]  B. P. Lewis,et al.  Thinking about Choking? Attentional Processes and Paradoxical Performance , 1997, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[51]  Michael Hoerger,et al.  Participant Dropout as a Function of Survey Length in Internet-Mediated University Studies: Implications for Study Design and Voluntary Participation in Psychological Research , 2010, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[52]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  More on the fragility of performance: choking under pressure in mathematical problem solving. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[53]  Joseph G. Johnson,et al.  Take The First: Option-generation and resulting choices , 2003 .

[54]  Thomas H. Carr,et al.  Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Memory Still Be Threatened? on the Causal Mechanisms of Stereotype Threat: Can Skills That Don't Rely Heavily on Working , 2022 .

[55]  Michael Hoerger,et al.  Affective forecasting and the Big Five. , 2010, Personality and individual differences.

[56]  R. Masters Theoretical aspects of implicit learning in sport. , 2000 .

[57]  R. Baumeister Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. , 1984, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[58]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .

[59]  N. Charness,et al.  Expert Performance Its Structure and Acquisition , 2002 .

[60]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Haste does not always make waste: Expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[61]  Michael Hoerger,et al.  Racial bias in personality assessment: using the MMPI-2 to predict psychiatric diagnoses of African American and Caucasian chemical dependency inpatients. , 2009, Psychological assessment.

[62]  Kelly J. Ashford,et al.  Attentional Focus, Dispositional Reinvestment, and Skilled Motor Performance under Pressure , 2006 .