Visual Cortex Extrastriate Body-Selective Area Activation in Congenitally Blind People “Seeing” by Using Sounds

Vision is by far the most prevalent sense for experiencing others' body shapes, postures, actions, and intentions, and its congenital absence may dramatically hamper body-shape representation in the brain. We investigated whether the absence of visual experience and limited exposure to others' body shapes could still lead to body-shape selectivity. We taught congenitally fully-blind adults to perceive full-body shapes conveyed through a sensory-substitution algorithm topographically translating images into soundscapes [1]. Despite the limited experience of the congenitally blind with external body shapes (via touch of close-by bodies and for ~10 hr via soundscapes), once the blind could retrieve body shapes via soundscapes, they robustly activated the visual cortex, specifically the extrastriate body area (EBA; [2]). Furthermore, body selectivity versus textures, objects, and faces in both the blind and sighted control groups was not found in the temporal (auditory) or parietal (somatosensory) cortex but only in the visual EBA. Finally, resting-state data showed that the blind EBA is functionally connected to the temporal cortex temporal-parietal junction/superior temporal sulcus Theory-of-Mind areas [3]. Thus, the EBA preference is present without visual experience and with little exposure to external body-shape information, supporting the view that the brain has a sensory-independent, task-selective supramodal organization rather than a sensory-specific organization.

[1]  P. Pietrini,et al.  Mind the blind brain to understand the sighted one! Is there a supramodal cortical functional architecture? , 2014, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[2]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[3]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Typical neural representations of action verbs develop without vision. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  How Many Subjects Constitute a Study? , 1999, NeuroImage.

[5]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The Representation of Tools in Left Parietal Cortex Is Independent of Visual Experience , 2010, Psychological science.

[6]  O. Blanke Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness , 2012, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[7]  G. Vandewalle,et al.  Functional specialization for auditory–spatial processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind humans , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  A. Amedi,et al.  The brain as a flexible task machine: implications for visual rehabilitation using noninvasive vs. invasive approaches. , 2012, Current opinion in neurology.

[9]  Peter B. L. Meijer,et al.  An experimental system for auditory image representations , 1992, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[10]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  The metamodal organization of the brain. , 2001, Progress in brain research.

[11]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Tool Selectivity in Left Occipitotemporal Cortex Develops without Vision , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  V. Slaughter,et al.  Developing expertise in human body perception , 2012 .

[13]  M. Costantini,et al.  Haptic perception and body representation in lateral and medial occipito-temporal cortices , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Amir Amedi,et al.  Reading with Sounds: Sensory Substitution Selectively Activates the Visual Word Form Area in the Blind , 2012, Neuron.

[15]  Shachar Maidenbaum,et al.  Author's Personal Copy Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Sensory Substitution: Closing the Gap between Basic Research and Widespread Practical Visual Rehabilitation Author's Personal Copy , 2022 .

[16]  P. Downing,et al.  Selectivity for the human body in the fusiform gyrus. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  Preserved Functional Specialization for Spatial Processing in the Middle Occipital Gyrus of the Early Blind , 2010, Neuron.

[18]  Rebecca F. Schwarzlose,et al.  Separate Face and Body Selectivity on the Fusiform Gyrus , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  P. Downing,et al.  The neural basis of visual body perception , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[20]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Growing up blind does not change the neural bases of Theory of Mind , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  Amir Amedi,et al.  ‘Visual’ Acuity of the Congenitally Blind Using Visual-to-Auditory Sensory Substitution , 2012, PloS one.

[22]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  A Cortical Area Selective for Visual Processing of the Human Body , 2001, Science.

[23]  Alfred Stett,et al.  Subretinal electronic chips allow blind patients to read letters and combine them to words , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  What drives the organization of object knowledge in the brain? , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  Emiliano Ricciardi,et al.  Beyond sensory images: Object-based representation in the human ventral pathway. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  A. Amedi,et al.  The large-scale organization of "visual" streams emerges without visual experience. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  David J. C. MacKay,et al.  Information Theory, Inference, and Learning Algorithms , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Information Theory.

[28]  Luciano Fadiga,et al.  Do We Really Need Vision? How Blind People “See” the Actions of Others , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Susan J. Lederman,et al.  Functional Specialization and Convergence in the Occipito-temporal Cortex Supporting Haptic and Visual Identification of Human Faces and Body Parts: An fMRI Study , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  I. Gauthier,et al.  Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  William M. Stern,et al.  Shape conveyed by visual-to-auditory sensory substitution activates the lateral occipital complex , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[33]  Ron Kupers,et al.  Navigation with a sensory substitution device in congenitally blind individuals , 2011, Neuroreport.

[34]  Teodora Gliga,et al.  Structural Encoding of Body and Face in Human Infants and Adults , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  M. Ptito,et al.  Recruitment of the middle temporal area by tactile motion in congenital blindness , 2009, Neuroreport.

[36]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Neural representations of faces and limbs neighbor in human high-level visual cortex: evidence for a new organization principle , 2011, Psychological Research.

[37]  Kalanit Grill-Spector,et al.  Not one extrastriate body area: Using anatomical landmarks, hMT+, and visual field maps to parcellate limb-selective activations in human lateral occipitotemporal cortex , 2011, NeuroImage.

[38]  M. Tsakiris My body in the brain: A neurocognitive model of body-ownership , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Category-Specific Organization in the Human Brain Does Not Require Visual Experience , 2009, Neuron.

[40]  L. Merabet,et al.  Neural reorganization following sensory loss: the opportunity of change , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[41]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Organization and Reorganization of Sensory-Deprived Cortex , 2012, Current Biology.

[42]  N. Kanwisher Functional specificity in the human brain: A window into the functional architecture of the mind , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Location and spatial profile of category‐specific regions in human extrastriate cortex , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[44]  T. Hendler,et al.  Visuo-haptic object-related activation in the ventral visual pathway , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  Valeria I. Petkova,et al.  Rubber Hands Feel Touch, but Not in Blind Individuals , 2012, PloS one.

[46]  B. Biswal,et al.  Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo‐planar mri , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[47]  M. Petrides,et al.  Neural correlates of mental transformations of the body-in-space. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.