Complex interaction of the sleep-wake cycle and circadian phase modulates mood in healthy subjects.

BACKGROUND Several studies of healthy volunteers have revealed that subjective mood may vary with the duration of prior wakefulness and with the time of day. However, in these studies, the effects of extended wakefulness and circadian phase remained confounded, and the interaction of these 2 processes could not be assessed quantitatively. METHODS In the present study, a total of 24 healthy young subjects (16 men, 8 women) lived on a 30-hour sleep-wake schedule for 19 to 23 days or on a 28-hour sleep-wake schedule for 33 to 36 days; both schedules induced desynchrony between the subjects' sleep-wake cycle and their endogenous circadian pacemaker. Subjective mood was assessed by 2 types of visual analog scales, which were administered twice every 2 hours and every 20 minutes, respectively, during all scheduled wakefulness episodes. A circadian phase and an interval elapsed since awakening were attributed to each data point, and circadian and wake-dependent fluctuations of mood were assessed. RESULTS A significant variation of mood with circadian phase was observed, but no reliable main effect of the duration of prior wakefulness was found. A statistically significant interaction of circadian and wake-dependent fluctuations was evident; when the analysis was restricted to specific circadian phases, mood improved, deteriorated, or remained stable with the duration of prior wakefulness. CONCLUSIONS These results indicate that, in healthy young subjects, subjective mood is influenced by a complex and nonadditive interaction of circadian phase and duration of prior wakefulness. The nature of this interaction is such that moderate changes in the timing of the sleep-wake cycle may have profound effects on subsequent mood.

[1]  P. Knauth,et al.  Desynchronization of different physiological functions during three weeks of experimental nightshift with limited and unlimited sleep , 1975 .

[2]  F. G. Spear,et al.  Diurnal variation of subjective mood in depressive states , 1964, The Psychiatric quarterly.

[3]  Z Vokac,et al.  Patterns and duration of sleep in permanent security night guards. , 1982, Journal of human ergology.

[4]  R. Berger,et al.  Performance and mood following variations in the length and timing of sleep. , 1973, Psychophysiology.

[5]  D. Beersma,et al.  On the contribution of sleep wake physiology to the explanation and the treatment of depression , 1988, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica. Supplementum.

[6]  A. Elithorn,et al.  Observations on some diurnal rhythms in depressive illness. , 1966, British medical journal.

[7]  M. Candito,et al.  Circadian rhythms in depression and recovery: Evidence for blunted amplitude as the main chronobiological abnormality , 1989, Psychiatry Research.

[8]  C. Hendrick,et al.  The structure of mood: a comparison between sleep deprivation and normal wakefulness conditions. , 1970, Journal of personality.

[9]  R. Kronauer,et al.  Exposure to bright light and darkness to treat physiologic maladaptation to night work. , 1990, The New England journal of medicine.

[10]  R. Post,et al.  Biological and behavioral effects of one night's sleep deprivation in depressed patients and normals. , 1979, Journal of psychiatric research.

[11]  D. Healy,et al.  Shiftwork, helplessness and depression. , 1993, Journal of affective disorders.

[12]  D.G.M. Dijk,et al.  Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat to sleep propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow waves, and sleep spindle activity in humans , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  F. Goodwin,et al.  Phase advance of the circadian sleep-wake cycle as an antidepressant. , 1979, Science.

[14]  M. Berger,et al.  Sleep deprivation with consecutive sleep-phase advance therapy in patients with major depression: A pilot study , 1993, Biological Psychiatry.

[15]  Simon Folkard,et al.  In situ repeated measures of affect and cognitive performance facilitated by use of a hand-held computer , 1992 .

[16]  A. Bouhuys Towards a model of mood responses to sleep deprivation in depressed patients , 1991, Biological Psychiatry.

[17]  D. Pringuey,et al.  Antidepressant effects of the sleep/wake cycle phase advance. Preliminary report. , 1987, Journal of affective disorders.

[18]  W S Myles,et al.  Effects of prolonged sleep deprivation, with and without chronic physical exercise, on mood and performance. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[19]  T. Åkerstedt,et al.  Towards the Prediction of Alertness on Abnormal Sleep/Wake Schedules , 1989 .

[20]  Derk-Jan Dijk,et al.  Entrained phase of the circadian pacemaker serves to stabilize alertness and performance throughout the habitual waking day. , 1994 .

[21]  R. Kronauer,et al.  Bright light induction of strong (type 0) resetting of the human circadian pacemaker. , 1989, Science.

[22]  David C. Klein,et al.  Suprachiasmatic nucleus : the mind's clock , 1991 .

[23]  C A Czeisler,et al.  Short‐term memory, alertness and performance: a reappraisal of their relationship to body temperature , 1992, Journal of sleep research.

[24]  S. Kasper,et al.  Therapeutic sleep deprivation and antidepressant medication in patients with major depression , 1991, European Neuropsychopharmacology.

[25]  A. Wirz-Justice,et al.  Diurnal variation of mood in depression: Important or irrelevant? , 1993, Biological Psychiatry.

[26]  R. Kronauer,et al.  Sensitivity of the Human Circadian Pacemaker to Moderately Bright Light , 1994, Journal of biological rhythms.

[27]  Maria L. Thomas,et al.  Effects of Catecholamine Depletion on Alertness and Mood in Rested and Sleep Deprived Normal Volunteers , 1993, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[28]  R. V. D. Hoofdakker Chronobiological theories of nonseasonal affective disorders and their implications for treatment. , 1994 .

[29]  J. Skipper,et al.  Nurses and shiftwork: effects on physical health and mental depression. , 1990, Journal of advanced nursing.

[30]  T. Wehr,et al.  Twenty-four-hour profiles of body temperature and plasma TSH in bipolar patients during depression and during remission and in normal control subjects. , 1988, The American journal of psychiatry.

[31]  J. Wu,et al.  The biological basis of an antidepressant response to sleep deprivation and relapse: review and hypothesis. , 1990, The American journal of psychiatry.

[32]  R. Kronauer,et al.  Simulations of light effects on the human circadian pacemaker: implications for assessment of intrinsic period. , 1996, The American journal of physiology.

[33]  T H Monk,et al.  Rhythmic vs homeostatic influences on mood, activation, and performance in young and old men. , 1992, Journal of gerontology.

[34]  R. Aitken Measurement of feelings using visual analogue scales. , 1969, Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[35]  F. G. Benedict STUDIES IN BODY-TEMPERATURE.— I. INFLUENCE OF THE INVERSION OF THE DAILY ROUTINE; THE TEMPERATURE OF NIGHT-WORKERS , 1904 .

[36]  J. Gillin The sleep therapies of depression , 1983, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[37]  A. Wirz-Justice,et al.  Prediction of the antidepressant response to total sleep deprivation by diurnal variation of mood , 1990, Psychiatry Research.

[38]  A. Borbély,et al.  Sleep, sleep deprivation and depression. A hypothesis derived from a model of sleep regulation. , 1982, Human neurobiology.

[39]  D. Healy,et al.  The circadian system and the therapeutics of the affective disorders. , 1995, Pharmacology & therapeutics.

[40]  D. Dijk,et al.  Circadian and sleep/wake dependent aspects of subjective alertness and cognitive performance , 1992, Journal of sleep research.

[41]  G. Vogel,et al.  Improvement of depression by REM sleep deprivation. New findings and a theory. , 1980, Archives of general psychiatry.

[42]  R B Briner,et al.  Associations of sleep with everyday mood, minor symptoms and social interaction experience. , 1994, Sleep.

[43]  R. H. van den Hoofdakker,et al.  Can non-REM sleep be depressogenic? , 1992, Journal of affective disorders.

[44]  A. Lewy,et al.  Antidepressant and circadian phase-shifting effects of light. , 1987, Science.

[45]  L. Baxter,et al.  Effects of partial sleep deprivation on the diurnal variation of mood and motor activity in major depression , 1991, Biological Psychiatry.

[46]  B. Pflug,et al.  Disturbance of the 24-hour rhythm in endogenous depression and the treatment of endogenous depression by sleep deprivation. , 1971, International pharmacopsychiatry.

[47]  T. Åkerstedt,et al.  Validation of the S and C components of the three-process model of alertness regulation. , 1995, Sleep.

[48]  Daniel J Buysse,et al.  Sleep stage physiology, mood, and vigilance responses to total sleep deprivation in healthy 80-year-olds and 20-year-olds. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[49]  C. Wood,et al.  Diurnal Changes in Perceptions of Energy and Mood , 1992, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[50]  Derk-Jan Dijk,et al.  Paradoxical timing of the circadian rhythm of sleep propensity serves to consolidate sleep and wakefulness in humans , 1994, Neuroscience Letters.

[51]  C A Czeisler,et al.  The prevalence and health impact of shiftwork. , 1986, American journal of public health.