Do task-irrelevant direction-associated motion verbs affect action planning? Evidence from a Stroop paradigm
暂无分享,去创建一个
Barbara Kaup | Martin Lachmair | Carolin Dudschig | Carolin Dudschig | Martin Lachmair | M. Filippis | I. Vega | Irmgard Vega | Monica Filippis | Barbara Kaup
[1] Jeffrey N Rouder,et al. Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[2] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[3] B. Kaup,et al. Root versus roof: automatic activation of location information during word processing , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[4] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. Motor Resonance and Linguistic Focus , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[5] J. Stroop. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .
[6] M. Costantini,et al. When objects are close to me: Affordances in the peripersonal space , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[7] R. Proctor,et al. Influence of Irrelevant Information on Human Performance: Effects of S-R Association Strength and Relative Timing , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[8] C. Buss,et al. Children's Brain Development Benefits from Longer Gestation , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[9] Lawrence W. Barsalou,et al. Perceptions of perceptual symbols , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[10] Ronald E. Shor,et al. Semantic gradients and interference in naming color, spatial direction, and numerosity , 1971 .
[11] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content , 2008, Journal of Physiology-Paris.
[12] Robert W. Proctor,et al. Influences of hand posture and hand position on compatibility effects for up-down stimuli mapped to left-right responses: Evidence for a hand referent hypothesis , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[13] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] George S. Cree,et al. Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words , 2008, Cognition.
[15] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[16] Harold Bekkering,et al. Context Effects in Embodied Lexical-Semantic Processing , 2010, Front. Psychology.
[17] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] Ronald E. Shor,et al. The processing of conceptual information on spatial directions from pictorial and linguistic symbols , 1970 .
[19] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[20] R. Proctor,et al. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[21] Donald R. Barr,et al. Using Confidence Intervals to Test Hypotheses , 1969 .
[22] M. de Vega,et al. Metaphors are Embodied, and so are Their Literal Counterparts , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[23] Daniel N. Bub,et al. On the nature of hand-action representations evoked during written sentence comprehension , 2010, Cognition.
[24] B. Rossion,et al. Fixation Patterns During Recognition of Personally Familiar and Unfamiliar Faces , 2010, Front. Psychology.
[25] Bahador Bahrami,et al. Visual motion interferes with lexical decision on motion words , 2008, Current Biology.
[26] D. Palermo,et al. The Nature and Ontogenesis of Meaning , 2023 .
[27] I. Johnsrude,et al. Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.
[28] Michael P. Kaschak,et al. Grounding language in action , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[29] Marc Jeannerod,et al. Cross-talk between Language Processes and Overt Motor Behavior in the First 200 msec of Processing , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[30] Daniel C. Richardson,et al. Spatial language, visual attention, and perceptual simulation , 2010, Brain and Language.
[31] P. König,et al. Testing the theory of embodied cognition with subliminal words , 2010, Cognition.
[32] R. Ratcliff. Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.
[33] Barbara Kaup. Word- vs. sentence-based simulation effects in language comprehension , 2012 .
[34] Livia Colle,et al. The neural basis for understanding non-intended actions , 2007, NeuroImage.
[35] L. Tyler,et al. Modulation of motor and premotor cortices by actions, action words and action sentences , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[36] M. Masson,et al. Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[37] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[38] Srini Narayanan,et al. Spatial and Linguistic Aspects of Visual Imagery in Sentence Comprehension , 2007, Cogn. Sci..
[39] M. Candidi,et al. Mapping Implied Body Actions in the Human Motor System , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[40] C. S. Englert,et al. Influence of Irrelevant Information in Addition Word Problems on Problem Solving , 1987 .
[41] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. Embodied sentence comprehension 1 EMBODIED SENTENCE COMPREHENSION , 2009 .