Exploring brain-behavior relationships in the N-back task

Working memory (WM) function has traditionally been investigated in terms of two dimensions: within-individual effects of WM load, and between-individual differences in task performance. In human neuroimaging studies, the N-back task has frequently been used to study both. A reliable finding is that activation in frontoparietal regions exhibits an inverted-U pattern, such that activity tends to decrease at high load levels. Yet it is not known whether such U-shaped patterns are a key individual differences factor that can predict load-related changes in task performance. The current study investigated this question by manipulating load levels across a much wider range than explored previously (N = 1-6), and providing a more comprehensive examination of brain-behavior relationships. In a sample of healthy young adults (n = 57), the analysis focused on a distinct region of left lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) identified in prior work to show a unique relationship with task performance and WM function. In this region it was the linear slope of load-related activity, rather than the U-shaped pattern that was positively associated with individual differences in target accuracy. Comprehensive supplemental analyses revealed the brain-wide selectivity of this pattern. Target accuracy was also independently predicted by the global resting-state connectivity of this LPFC region. These effects were robust, as demonstrated by cross-validation analyses and out-of-sample prediction, and also critically, were primarily driven by the high-load conditions. Together, the results highlight the utility of high-load conditions for investigating individual differences in WM function.

[1]  R. Gur,et al.  Development of Abbreviated Nine-Item Forms of the Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices Test , 2012, Assessment.

[2]  Michael W. L. Chee,et al.  Dissociation of cortical regions modulated by both working memory load and sleep deprivation and by sleep deprivation alone , 2005, NeuroImage.

[3]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  On how high performers keep cool brains in situations of cognitive overload , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[4]  Tobias Otto,et al.  Neural correlates of mental effort evaluation--involvement of structures related to self-awareness. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[5]  Xiao-Li Meng,et al.  Comparing correlated correlation coefficients , 1992 .

[6]  Christa Neuper,et al.  Intelligence and neural efficiency: further evidence of the influence of task content and sex on the brain-IQ relationship. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[7]  Richard Levy,et al.  Cognitive control and brain resources in major depression: An fMRI study using the n-back task , 2005, NeuroImage.

[8]  Theo G. M. van Erp,et al.  The relationship between performance and fMRI signal during working memory in patients with schizophrenia, unaffected co-twins, and control subjects , 2007, Schizophrenia Research.

[9]  Ion Juvina,et al.  Modeling Control Strategies in the N-Back Task , 2007 .

[10]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Diffusion Decision Model: Current Issues and History , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  Andreas Papassotiropoulos,et al.  Identification of Two Distinct Working Memory-Related Brain Networks in Healthy Young Adults , 2018, eNeuro.

[12]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory , 2005, Nature.

[13]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[14]  T. Wickens Elementary Signal Detection Theory , 2001 .

[15]  E. Rolls,et al.  Holding Multiple Items in Short Term Memory: A Neural Mechanism , 2013, PloS one.

[16]  R. Coppola,et al.  Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  N. Fox,et al.  NIH Toolbox for Assessment of Neurological and Behavioral Function , 2013, Neurology.

[18]  Timothy O. Laumann,et al.  Functional Network Organization of the Human Brain , 2011, Neuron.

[19]  Cleofé Peña-Gómez,et al.  Brain connectivity during resting state and subsequent working memory task predicts behavioural performance , 2012, Cortex.

[20]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  From Distributed Resources to Limited Slots in Multiple-Item Working Memory: A Spiking Network Model with Normalization , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  T. Yarkoni Psychoinformatics: New Horizons at the Interface of the Psychological and Computing Sciences , 2012 .

[22]  T. Braver,et al.  What Is the Subjective Cost of Cognitive Effort? Load, Trait, and Aging Effects Revealed by Economic Preference , 2013, PloS one.

[23]  Patrick C. Kyllonen,et al.  Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! , 1990 .

[24]  Michael F. Bunting,et al.  Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[25]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  Mapping the human brain's cortical-subcortical functional network organization , 2018, NeuroImage.

[26]  J. Jonides,et al.  Brain mechanisms of proactive interference in working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.

[27]  K. Ewing,et al.  T he impact of wor king memory load on psychophysiological measures of mental effort and motivational disposition , 2010 .

[28]  Theo G. M. van Erp,et al.  Re-evaluating dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation during working memory in schizophrenia , 2009, Schizophrenia Research.

[29]  Jesper Tegnér,et al.  Mechanism for top-down control of working memory capacity , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[30]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.

[31]  A. Gevins,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of component processes in human working memory. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[32]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Neural correlates of variable working memory load across adult age and skill: dissociative patterns within the fronto-parietal network. , 2009, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[33]  Daphna Shohamy,et al.  Dynamic shifts in brain network activation during supracapacity working memory task performance , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[34]  N. Ramsey,et al.  Working memory capacity in schizophrenia: a parametric fMRI study , 2004, Schizophrenia Research.

[35]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  The neural bases of distracter-resistant working memory , 2011, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[36]  Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Age differences in prefontal recruitment during verbal working memory maintenance depend on memory load , 2010, Cortex.

[37]  E. Vogel,et al.  Visual working memory capacity: from psychophysics and neurobiology to individual differences , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  Kathryn M. McMillan,et al.  N‐back working memory paradigm: A meta‐analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[39]  T. Braver,et al.  The Subjective Value of Cognitive Effort is Encoded by a Domain-General Valuation Network , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Hang Joon Jo,et al.  Mapping sources of correlation in resting state FMRI, with artifact detection and removal , 2010, NeuroImage.

[41]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  A Parametric Study of Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Human Working Memory , 1996, NeuroImage.

[42]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  Function in the human connectome: Task-fMRI and individual differences in behavior , 2013, NeuroImage.

[43]  T. Yarkoni,et al.  Choosing Prediction Over Explanation in Psychology: Lessons From Machine Learning , 2017, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[44]  P. Ackerman,et al.  Working Memory and Intelligence : The Same or Different Constructs ? , 2005 .

[45]  M. Buchsbaum,et al.  Intelligence and changes in regional cerebral glucose metabolic rate following learning , 1992 .

[46]  Lucina Q. Uddin,et al.  Combining region- and network-level brain-behavior relationships in a structural equation model , 2018, NeuroImage.

[47]  C. Bundesen,et al.  Changing change detection: Improving the reliability of measures of visual short-term memory capacity , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[48]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[49]  Angela R. Laird,et al.  Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate-based meta-analysis , 2012, NeuroImage.

[50]  Randall W Engle,et al.  Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[51]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  Activity flow over resting-state networks shapes cognitive task activations , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  Lateral Prefrontal Cortex Contributes to Fluid Intelligence Through Multinetwork Connectivity , 2015, Brain Connect..

[53]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  Global Connectivity of Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Cognitive Control and Intelligence , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  W. Revelle psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research , 2017 .

[55]  R. Engle,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[56]  Seth A. Herd,et al.  From an Executive Network to Executive Control: A Computational Model of the n-back Task , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.