Perceptually Grounded Faithfulness in Harmonic Serialism

Steriade (2008) argues that faithfulness constraints in Optimality Theory are perceptually grounded: the faithfulness of a phonological mapping is directly proportional to the perceptual similarity between the input and the output of that mapping. For example, when a phonological process like place assimilation, voice assimilation, or deletion affects a medial consonant cluster /VC1C2V/, it usually targets C1 rather than C2. The explanation is that changing C1 is less of a change perceptually than changing C2, because C2’s prevocalic position gives it stronger perceptual cues than C1. Formally, this difference in strength of perceptual cues is reflected in the ranking of faithfulness constraints: processes affecting C1 violate lower-ranking faithfulness constraints than processes affecting C2. A mapping has an input and an output, and faithfulness constraints require an input and an output to compare. In the standard parallel version of Optimality Theory, referred to here as P-OT, the input to every mapping is the underlying representation and the output is the surface representation (Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004). When this property of P-OT is combined with perceptually grounded faithfulness, a problem arises (Blumenfeld 2006, Flemming 2006, 2008a,b, Gallagher 2006, Jun 2002, McCarthy 2008b, Wilson 2001): underlying representations lack information that is important for perception. For example, the release of a stop consonant contains important perceptual cues for its place of articulation. In most if not all languages, the distribution of release is determined by the grammar, not the lexicon: stops are released prevocalically, but they may be unreleased (depending on the language) preconsonantally. If important perceptual cues like release are not already determined in underlying representation, how can faithfulness be perceptually grounded?

[1]  D. R. Ladd,et al.  Phonetics in Phonology , 2011 .

[2]  J. Ohala Papers in Laboratory Phonology: The phonetics and phonology of aspects of assimilation , 1990 .

[3]  Donca Steriade,et al.  ALTERNATIVES TO SYLLABLE-BASED ACCOUNTS OF CONSONANTAL PHONOTACTICS , 2000 .

[4]  John Kingston,et al.  Papers in Laboratory Phonology: Index of names , 1990 .

[5]  Martin Krämer,et al.  Freedom of analysis , 2007 .

[6]  John Goldsmith,et al.  The last phonological rule : reflections on constraints and derivations , 1993 .

[7]  John J. McCarthy,et al.  The gradual path to cluster simplification* , 2008, Phonology.

[8]  Edward Flemming,et al.  DRAFT 5 / 9 / 06 1 The role of distinctiveness constraints in phonology , 2006 .

[9]  John J. McCarthy,et al.  An Introduction to Harmonic Serialism , 2010, Lang. Linguistics Compass.

[10]  CHARLOTTE WEBB,et al.  A constraint on progressive consonantal assimilation , 1982 .

[11]  J. McCarthy Slouching toward optimality: Coda reduction in OT-CC , 2007 .

[12]  Edward Flemming Asymmetries between assimilation and epenthesis , 2008 .

[13]  Loren F. Bliese A generative grammar of Afar , 1981 .

[14]  B. J. Hoff The Carib Language: Phonology, Morphonology, Morphology, Texts and Word Index , 1968 .

[15]  Jongho Jun Positional Faithfulness, Sympathy, and Inferred Input , 2005 .

[16]  John J. McCarthy,et al.  Harmonic Serialism and Parallelism , 2000 .

[17]  전종호 Hidden Generalizations: Phonological Opacity in Optimality Theory , 2007 .

[18]  Colin Wilson,et al.  Consonant cluster neutralisation and targeted constraints , 2001, Phonology.

[19]  Mairo E. Kidda Tangale phonology : a descriptive analysis , 1993 .

[20]  Junko Ito,et al.  A prosodic theory of epenthesis , 1989 .

[21]  Spike Gildea,et al.  A Comparative Description of Syllable Reduction in the Cariban Language Family , 1995, International Journal of American Linguistics.

[22]  John J. McCarthy,et al.  Restraint of Analysis , 2006 .

[23]  John J. McCarthy,et al.  Doing Optimality Theory , 2008 .

[24]  Wendell A. Kimper Locality and globality in phonological variation , 2011 .

[25]  J. Goldsmith Autosegmental and Metrical Phonology , 1990 .

[26]  D. Steriade Phonetics in Phonology: The Case of Laryngeal Neutralization , 1999 .

[27]  Keith Johnson,et al.  The Role of Speech Perception in Phonology , 2001 .

[28]  John J. McCarthy,et al.  Wondering at the Natural Fecundity of Things: Essays in Honor of Alan Prince , 2006 .

[29]  Robert Kirchner,et al.  Phonetically Based Phonology , 2008 .

[30]  William S-Y. Wang,et al.  13 Vowel Features, Paired Variables, and the English Vowel Shift , 1968 .

[31]  P. Smolensky,et al.  Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar , 2004 .

[32]  Jongho Jun,et al.  Perceptual and articulatory factors in place assimilation : an optimality theoretic approach , 1995 .

[33]  Donca Steriade,et al.  The Phonology of Perceptibility Effects: The P-Map and Its Consequences for Constraint Organization , 2008 .

[34]  R. Wilkinson A Phonetic Constraint on a Syncope Rule in Telugu , 1974 .

[35]  William L. Wonderly Zoque II: Phonemes and Morphophonemes , 1951, International Journal of American Linguistics.

[36]  Harry Hoijer,et al.  Linguistic structures of native America , 1965 .

[37]  Karen Jesney Positional Faithfulness, Non-locality, and the Harmonic Serialism Solution , 2011 .

[38]  John J. McCarthy,et al.  The serial interaction of stress and syncope , 2008 .

[39]  John J. McCarthy,et al.  A Thematic Guide to Optimality Theory , 2001 .

[40]  L. Lombardi Positional Faithfulness and Voicing Assimilation in Optimality Theory , 1999 .

[41]  Sharon Inkelas,et al.  The nature of the word : studies in honor of Paul Kiparsky , 2008 .