Interactive rhythms across species: the evolutionary biology of animal chorusing and turn‐taking

The study of human language is progressively moving toward comparative and interactive frameworks, extending the concept of turn‐taking to animal communication. While such an endeavor will help us understand the interactive origins of language, any theoretical account for cross‐species turn‐taking should consider three key points. First, animal turn‐taking must incorporate biological studies on animal chorusing, namely how different species coordinate their signals over time. Second, while concepts employed in human communication and turn‐taking, such as intentionality, are still debated in animal behavior, lower level mechanisms with clear neurobiological bases can explain much of animal interactive behavior. Third, social behavior, interactivity, and cooperation can be orthogonal, and the alternation of animal signals need not be cooperative. Considering turn‐taking a subset of chorusing in the rhythmic dimension may avoid overinterpretation and enhance the comparability of future empirical work.

[1]  Michael D Greenfield,et al.  Acoustic Dueling in Tarbush Grasshoppers: Settlement of Territorial Contests via Alternation of Reliable Signals , 2010 .

[2]  T. U. Grafe,et al.  The function of call alternation in the African reed frog (Hyperolius marmoratus): precise call timing prevents auditory masking , 1996, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[3]  Nicola S. Clayton,et al.  Intelligence in Corvids and Apes: A Case of Convergent Evolution? , 2009 .

[4]  Michael D Greenfield Signalers and Receivers: Mechanisms and Evolution of Arthropod Communication , 2002 .

[5]  S. Kotz,et al.  Putting language back into ecological communication contexts , 2018, Language, Cognition and Neuroscience.

[6]  Michael D Greenfield,et al.  Phylogeographic structure without pre-mating barriers: Do habitat fragmentation and low mobility preserve song and chorus diversity in a European bushcricket? , 2017, Evolutionary Ecology.

[7]  J. Buck,et al.  Mechanism of Rhythmic Synchronous Flashing of Fireflies , 1968, Science.

[8]  Elma E. Hilbrink,et al.  Early developmental changes in the timing of turn-taking: a longitudinal study of mother–infant interaction , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[9]  S. Levinson,et al.  Turn-timing in signed conversations: coordinating stroke-to-stroke turn boundaries , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[10]  V. Nityananda Attention-like processes in insects , 2016, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[11]  Francisco Torreira,et al.  Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[12]  A. Dickinson,et al.  The Intentionality of Animal Action , 1990 .

[13]  S. Levinson,et al.  Multimodal Language Processing in Human Communication , 2019, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  R. Hayashi,et al.  Simultaneous talk—from the perspective of floor management of English and Japanese speakers , 1988 .

[15]  Michael D Greenfield,et al.  Cooperation and Conflict in the Evolution of Signal Interactions , 1994 .

[16]  Klaus Zuberbühler,et al.  Chimpanzee Alarm Call Production Meets Key Criteria for Intentionality , 2013, PloS one.

[17]  Kazuyuki Aihara,et al.  Mathematical modeling of frogs’ calling behavior and its possible application to artificial life and robotics , 2008, Artificial Life and Robotics.

[18]  Cristina Giacoma,et al.  The Indris Have Got Rhythm! Timing and Pitch Variation of a Primate Song Examined between Sexes and Age Classes , 2016, Front. Neurosci..

[19]  A. Ghazanfar,et al.  Early development of turn-taking with parents shapes vocal acoustics in infant marmoset monkeys , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  吉田 重人,et al.  Evolution of Turn-Taking: A Bio-Cognitive Perspective (特集 動物の認知) , 2005 .

[21]  Cognitive Ethology and the Intentionality of Animal Behaviour , 1995 .

[22]  H. Kokko Give one species the task to come up with a theory that spans them all: what good can come out of that? , 2017, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  Cory T. Miller,et al.  Vocal turn-taking in a non-human primate is learned during ontogeny , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  Why only us: Recent questions and answers , 2017, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[25]  Michael D Greenfield,et al.  Synchronous and Alternating Choruses in Insects and Anurans: Common Mechanisms and Diverse Functions , 1994 .

[26]  Steffen R. Hage,et al.  Limited capabilities for condition-dependent modulation of vocal turn-taking behavior in marmoset monkeys. , 2019, Behavioral neuroscience.

[27]  H. Römer,et al.  Rhythm Generation and Rhythm Perception in Insects: The Evolution of Synchronous Choruses , 2016, Front. Neurosci..

[28]  Kazuyuki Aihara,et al.  Spatio-Temporal Dynamics in Collective Frog Choruses Examined by Mathematical Modeling and Field Observations , 2014, Scientific Reports.

[29]  Barbara Webb,et al.  Cognition in insects , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[30]  E. Large,et al.  Beat Keeping in a Sea Lion As Coupled Oscillation: Implications for Comparative Understanding of Human Rhythm , 2016, Front. Neurosci..

[31]  W. Strange Evolution of language. , 1984, JAMA.

[32]  Toshio Aoyagi,et al.  Interaction mechanisms quantified from dynamical features of frog choruses , 2019, Royal Society Open Science.

[33]  David Poeppel,et al.  Cortical oscillations and speech processing: emerging computational principles and operations , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  S. Shettleworth Animal cognition and animal behaviour , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[35]  John R. Krebs,et al.  The Evolution of Alarm Calls: Altruism or Manipulation? , 1975, The American Naturalist.

[36]  M. Manser,et al.  Vocal Turn-Taking in Meerkat Group Calling Sessions , 2018, Current Biology.

[37]  W. Fitch,et al.  Monkey vocal tracts are speech-ready , 2016, Science Advances.

[38]  K. Wells The social behaviour of anuran amphibians , 1977, Animal Behaviour.

[39]  W. Fitch Nano-intentionality: a defense of intrinsic intentionality , 2008 .

[40]  Manfred Hartbauer,et al.  Mechanisms for synchrony and alternation in song interactions of the bushcricket Mecopoda elongata (Tettigoniidae: Orthoptera) , 2005, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[41]  M. Wittmann The inner sense of time: how the brain creates a representation of duration , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[42]  W. Tecumseh Fitch,et al.  Chorusing, synchrony, and the evolutionary functions of rhythm , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[43]  R. Byrne Imitation without intentionality. Using string parsing to copy the organization of behaviour , 1999, Animal Cognition.

[44]  A. Ghazanfar,et al.  Coupled Oscillator Dynamics of Vocal Turn-Taking in Monkeys , 2013, Current Biology.

[45]  B. Webb Neural mechanisms for prediction: do insects have forward models? , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[46]  J. Ulijn,et al.  Is interrupting impolite? Some temporal aspects of turn-taking in Chinese-Western and other intercultural business encounters , 1995 .

[47]  Hugh J. McCann Intrinsic intentionality , 1986 .

[48]  D. Valente,et al.  Female indris determine the rhythmic structure of the song and sustain a higher cost when the chorus size increases , 2018, Current zoology.

[49]  Kobin H. Kendrick,et al.  Taking turns: bridging the gap between human and animal communication , 2018, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[50]  A. Wing,et al.  Optimal feedback correction in string quartet synchronization , 2014, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[51]  L. Pozzi,et al.  An intra-population analysis of the indris’ song dissimilarity in the light of genetic distance , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[52]  Roger K. Moore Vocal Interactivity in Crowds, Flocks and Swarms: Implications for Voice User Interfaces , 2019, ArXiv.

[53]  Arthur T. Winfree,et al.  The timing of biological clocks , 1986 .

[54]  W. T. Fitch,et al.  The Evolution of Rhythm Processing , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  Dena J. Clink,et al.  Vocal individuality and rhythm in male and female duet contributions of a nonhuman primate , 2019, Current zoology.

[56]  J. Buck Synchronous Rhythmic Flashing of Fireflies , 1938, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[57]  E. Smith Communication and collective action: language and the evolution of human cooperation , 2010 .

[58]  Luc H. Arnal,et al.  Proactive Sensing of Periodic and Aperiodic Auditory Patterns , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[59]  Asif A. Ghazanfar,et al.  The evolution of speech: vision, rhythm, cooperation , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  N. Mann,et al.  Antiphonal four-part synchronized chorusing in a Neotropical wren , 2006, Biology Letters.

[61]  Andrea Ravignani,et al.  Modelling Animal Interactive Rhythms in Communication , 2019, Evolutionary bioinformatics online.

[62]  Margaret Wilson,et al.  An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[63]  J. Buck Synchronous Rhythmic Flashing of Fireflies. II. , 1938, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[64]  Xiaoqin Wang,et al.  Sensory-motor interaction in the primate auditory cortex during self-initiated vocalizations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  W. Meck,et al.  The Socio-Temporal Brain: Connecting People in Time , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[66]  M. K. Tourtellot,et al.  Precedence effects and the evolution of chorusing , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[67]  Luc H. Arnal,et al.  Transitions in neural oscillations reflect prediction errors generated in audiovisual speech , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[68]  K. Okanoya,et al.  Temporal adjustment of short calls according to a partner during vocal turn-taking in Japanese macaques , 2018, Current zoology.

[69]  J. Greenlee,et al.  Human Auditory Cortical Activation during Self-Vocalization , 2011, PloS one.

[70]  Kobin H. Kendrick,et al.  Editorial: Turn-Taking in Human Communicative Interaction , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[71]  S. Malaivijitnond,et al.  Male white-handed gibbons flexibly time duet contributions , 2017, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[72]  Piera Filippi Emotional and Interactional Prosody across Animal Communication Systems: A Comparative Approach to the Emergence of Language , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[73]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The language capacity: architecture and evolution , 2016, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[74]  Michael D Greenfield,et al.  Katydid synchronous chorusing is an evolutionarily stable outcome of female choice , 1993, Nature.

[75]  A. Arak Vocal interactions, call matching and territoriality in a Sri Lankan treefrog, Philautus leucorhinus (Rhacophoridae) , 1983, Animal Behaviour.

[76]  Heidi Byrnes,et al.  Interactional style in German and American conversations , 1986 .

[77]  D. Kelley,et al.  Rapping, a female receptive call, initiates male-female duets in the South African clawed frog. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[78]  P. Gärdenfors,et al.  Attention as a minimal criterion of intentionality in robots , 2002 .

[79]  P. Kay,et al.  Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[80]  M. Pickering,et al.  Early preparation during turn-taking: Listeners use content predictions to determine what to say but not when to say it , 2018, Cognition.

[81]  J. M. Smith,et al.  The Evolution of Alarm Calls , 1965, The American Naturalist.

[82]  P. Turner,et al.  Evolution of mutualism from parasitism in experimental virus populations , 2018, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[83]  Michael D Greenfield Signal interactions and interference in insect choruses: singing and listening in the social environment , 2014, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[84]  Andrew M. M. Matheson,et al.  Motor cortical control of vocal interaction in neotropical singing mice , 2019, Science.

[85]  S. Kotz,et al.  Evolution of temporal interaction: A comparative approach to social timing , 2017 .

[86]  S. Pika,et al.  Unpeeling the layers of language: Bonobos and chimpanzees engage in cooperative turn-taking sequences , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[87]  J. Gould,et al.  Population dynamics of Siphoninus philyreae in California in the presence and absence of a parasitoid, Encarsia partenopea , 1992 .

[88]  Lars Chittka,et al.  What is comparable in comparative cognition? , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[89]  Kobin H. Kendrick,et al.  Turn-Taking in Human Communicative Interaction , 2016 .

[90]  A. Ravignani Timing of Antisynchronous Calling: A Case Study in a Harbor Seal Pup (Phoca vitulina) , 2019, Journal of comparative psychology.

[91]  P. Backwell Synchronous waving in fiddler crabs: a review , 2018, Current zoology.

[92]  Christopher T. Kello,et al.  Rhythm in speech and animal vocalizations: a cross‐species perspective , 2019, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[93]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Why can't you tickle yourself? , 2000, Neuroreport.

[94]  S. Kirby,et al.  The psychology of biological clocks: A new framework for the evolution of rhythm , 2014 .

[95]  T. Grafe A function of synchronous chorusing and a novel female preference shift in an anuran , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[96]  Bill Thompson,et al.  The Evolution of Musicality: What Can Be Learned from Language Evolution Research? , 2018, Front. Neurosci..

[97]  W. Fitch,et al.  Social Origins of Rhythm? Synchrony and Temporal Regularity in Human Vocalization , 2013, PloS one.