Brain Substrates of Recovery from Misleading Influence

Humans are strongly influenced by their environment, a dependence that can lead to errors in judgment. Although a rich literature describes how people are influenced by others, little is known regarding the factors that predict subsequent rectification of misleading influence. Using a mediation model in combination with brain imaging, we propose a model for the correction of misinformation. Specifically, our data suggest that amygdala modulation of hippocampal mnemonic representations, during the time of misleading social influence, is associated with reduced subsequent anterior–lateral prefrontal cortex activity that reflects correction. These findings illuminate the process by which erroneous beliefs are, or fail to be, rectified and highlight how past influence constrains subsequent correction.

[1]  T. Sharot,et al.  The Brain's Temporal Dynamics from a Collective Decision to Individual Action , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Keith Bush,et al.  A comparison of statistical methods for detecting context-modulated functional connectivity in fMRI , 2014, NeuroImage.

[3]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  The Behavioral and Neural Mechanisms Underlying the Tracking of Expertise , 2013, Neuron.

[4]  E. Fehr,et al.  Changing Social Norm Compliance with Noninvasive Brain Stimulation , 2013, Science.

[5]  A. Hayes Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach , 2013 .

[6]  Lorena Deuker,et al.  Playing nice: a multi-methodological study on the effects of social conformity on memory , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[7]  D. Haun,et al.  Majority influence in children and other animals , 2013, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  D. Schacter,et al.  Memory and law: what can cognitive neuroscience contribute? , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  R. Dolan,et al.  Confidence in value-based choice , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  R. Kana,et al.  Neural representations of self versus other: Lessons from autism , 2013 .

[11]  J. Engelmann,et al.  Contextual and social influences on valuation and choice. , 2013, Progress in brain research.

[12]  Eleanor A. Maguire,et al.  Decoding information in the human hippocampus: A user's guide , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  Multi-voxel pattern analysis in human hippocampal subfields , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  R. Dolan,et al.  Prefrontal Contributions to Metacognition in Perceptual Decision Making , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Nicole M. Long,et al.  Supplemental Figure , 2013 .

[16]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Deconvolving BOLD activation in event-related designs for multivoxel pattern classification analyses , 2012, NeuroImage.

[17]  W. Hirst,et al.  Remembering in conversations: the social sharing and reshaping of memories. , 2012, Annual review of psychology.

[18]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Does the brain calculate value? , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  Mara Mather,et al.  Updating Existing Emotional Memories Involves the Frontopolar/Orbito-frontal Cortex in Ways that Acquiring New Emotional Memories Does Not , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  Scott A. Guerin,et al.  Memory distortion: an adaptive perspective , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  Yadin Dudai,et al.  Following the Crowd: Brain Substrates of Long-Term Memory Conformity , 2011, Science.

[22]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Review Frontal Cortex and Reward-guided Learning and Decision-making Figure 1. Frontal Brain Regions in the Macaque Involved in Reward-guided Learning and Decision-making Finer Grained Anatomical Divisions with Frontal Cortical Systems for Reward-guided Behavior , 2022 .

[23]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Social Influence Modulates the Neural Computation of Value , 2011, Psychological science.

[24]  Ana Raposo,et al.  Contributions of frontopolar cortex to judgments about self, others and relations. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[25]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Counterfactual Choice and Learning in a Neural Network Centered on Human Lateral Frontopolar Cortex , 2011, PLoS biology.

[26]  N. Bolger,et al.  Brain Mediators of Predictive Cue Effects on Perceived Pain , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Relating Introspective Accuracy to Individual Differences in Brain Structure , 2010, Science.

[28]  R. Dolan,et al.  How the Opinion of Others Affects Our Valuation of Objects , 2010, Current Biology.

[29]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Changing your mind: a computational mechanism of vacillation , 2009, Nature.

[30]  Daniel B. Wright,et al.  When Eyewitnesses Talk , 2009 .

[31]  Carter Wendelken,et al.  Left, but not right, rostrolateral prefrontal cortex meets a stringent test of the relational integration hypothesis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[32]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  Mahzarin R. Banaji,et al.  Neural Correlates of Stereotype Application , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  G. Fernández,et al.  Reinforcement Learning Signal Predicts Social Conformity , 2009, Neuron.

[35]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  From Fear to Safety and Back: Reversal of Fear in the Human Brain , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Brent L. Hughes,et al.  Prefrontal-Subcortical Pathways Mediating Successful Emotion Regulation , 2008, Neuron.

[37]  Y. Dudai,et al.  Mesmerizing Memories: Brain Substrates of Episodic Memory Suppression in Posthypnotic Amnesia , 2008, Neuron.

[38]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Anterior Prefrontal Function and the Limits of Human Decision-Making , 2007, Science.

[39]  U. Fischbacher,et al.  The Neural Signature of Social Norm Compliance , 2007, Neuron.

[40]  E. Phelps,et al.  Social learning of fear , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  Edmund A. Mennis The Wisdom of Crowds: Why the Many Are Smarter than the Few and How Collective Wisdom Shapes Business, Economies, Societies, and Nations , 2006 .

[42]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[43]  Nadim Joni Shah,et al.  Neural Representations of Self versus Other: Visual-Spatial Perspective Taking and Agency in a Virtual Ball-tossing Game , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  Stephen Lawrie,et al.  Functional Specialization within Rostral Prefrontal Cortex (Area 10): A Meta-analysis , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  Caroline F. Zink,et al.  Neurobiological Correlates of Social Conformity and Independence During Mental Rotation , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[46]  W. Hirst,et al.  How eyewitnesses resist misinformation: Social postwarnings and the monitoring of memory characteristics , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[47]  Dick J. Veltman,et al.  Neural correlates of a reversal learning task with an affectively neutral baseline: An event-related fMRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[48]  Denise C. Park,et al.  How warnings about false claims become recommendations , 2005 .

[49]  Rebecca Elliott,et al.  Role of the orbitofrontal cortex in reinforcement processing and inhibitory control: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging studies in healthy human subjects. , 2005, International review of neurobiology.

[50]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Reality Monitoring , 2005 .

[51]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  Measuring functional connectivity during distinct stages of a cognitive task , 2004, NeuroImage.

[52]  T. Robbins,et al.  The neuropsychology of ventral prefrontal cortex: Decision-making and reversal learning , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[53]  R. J Dolan,et al.  Dissociable Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Responses during Reversal Fear Conditioning , 2022 .

[54]  A. Owen,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[55]  Anthony D. Wagner,et al.  Cognitive Control, Semantic Memory, and Priming: Contributions from Prefrontal Cortex. , 2004 .

[56]  Robin I. M. Dunbar The Social Brain: Mind, Language, and Society in Evolutionary Perspective , 2003 .

[57]  Morten L Kringelbach,et al.  Neural correlates of rapid reversal learning in a simple model of human social interaction , 2003, NeuroImage.

[58]  Paul J. Laurienti,et al.  An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets , 2003, NeuroImage.

[59]  Henry L Roediger,et al.  Explorations in the social contagion of memory , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[60]  M. Potter,et al.  Constrained Formation of Object Representations , 2002, Psychological science.

[61]  M. Zaragoza,et al.  Intended and unintended effects of explicit warnings on eyewitness suggestibility: Evidence from source identification tests , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[62]  Ralph Hertwig,et al.  The Costs of Deception: Evidence from Psychology , 2001 .

[63]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex , 2000, Psychobiology.

[64]  Endel Tulving,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval mode. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[65]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting Cortical and Subcortical Activations Produced by Attentional-Set Shifting and Reversal Learning in Humans , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[66]  R. Adolphs Social cognition and the human brain , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[67]  Elizabeth F. Loftus,et al.  Advertising's misinformation effect , 1998 .

[68]  M. Farah,et al.  Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[69]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition: An Empirical Review of PET Studies with Normal Subjects , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  J C Mazziotta,et al.  Automated labeling of the human brain: A preliminary report on the development and evaluation of a forward‐transform method , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[71]  Malcolm C. Rubel Encoding information , 1993 .

[72]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Source monitoring. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[73]  R. Byrne,et al.  Machiavellian intelligence : social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans , 1990 .

[74]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[75]  David J. Stang,et al.  Ineffective deception in conformity research: Some causes and consequences , 1976 .

[76]  N. Humphrey The Social Function of Intellect , 1976 .

[77]  Mark R. Lepper,et al.  Perseverance in self-perception and social perception: biased attributional processes in the debriefing paradigm. , 1975, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[78]  Vernon L. Allen,et al.  Social Support for Nonconformity , 1975 .

[79]  D. Vickers,et al.  Evidence for an accumulator model of psychophysical discrimination. , 1970, Ergonomics.

[80]  M. Deutsch,et al.  A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgement. , 1955, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[81]  S. Asch Group forces in the modification and distortion of judgments. , 1952 .

[82]  F. H. Hankins,et al.  The Psychology of Social Norms , 1937 .

[83]  L. Doob The psychology of social norms. , 1937 .