Reaching to recover balance in unpredictable circumstances: Is online visual control of the reach-to-grasp reaction necessary or sufficient?

Reaching to grasp an object for support is a common and functionally important response to sudden balance perturbation. The need to react very rapidly (to prevent falling) imposes temporal constraints on acquisition and processing of the visuospatial information (VSI) needed to guide the reaching movement. Previous results suggested that the CNS may deal with these constraints by using VSI stored in memory proactively, prior to perturbation onset; however, the extent to which online visual control is necessary or sufficient to guide these reactions has not been established. This study examined the speed, accuracy, and effectiveness of perturbation-evoked reach-to-grasp reactions when forced to rely entirely on either online- or stored-VSI by using liquid–crystal goggles to occlude vision either before or after perturbation onset. The reactions were evoked, in twelve healthy young adults, via sudden unpredictable antero-posterior platform translation (barriers deterred stepping reactions). Prior to perturbation onset, a small cylindrical handhold was positioned unpredictably (by a motor-driven device) at one of four locations in front of the subject. Results indicated that equilibrium could be recovered successfully by grasping the handhold using either online-VSI or stored-VSI to guide the arm reaction; however, both sources of VSI were required for optimal performance. Reach initiation and arm movement were slowed when dependent on online-VSI, whereas reach accuracy and grip formation were impaired when dependent on stored-VSI. Comparison with normal-VSI trials suggests that both sources of VSI are utilized when grasping a small handhold for support under normal visual conditions, with stored-VSI predominating during initiation/transport and online-VSI contributing primarily to final target acquisition/prehension.

[1]  H Carnahan,et al.  The temporal organization of hand, eye, and head movements during reaching and pointing. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.

[2]  R. Iman,et al.  Rank Transformations as a Bridge between Parametric and Nonparametric Statistics , 1981 .

[3]  M. Goodale,et al.  The effects of delay on the kinematics of grasping , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  B. E. Maki,et al.  Influence of lateral destabilization on compensatory stepping responses. , 1996, Journal of biomechanics.

[5]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of uncertainty and premovement visual information on manual aiming. , 1990, Canadian journal of psychology.

[6]  Ian M Franks,et al.  The utilization of visual feedback in the control of movement direction: evidence from a video aiming task. , 2003, Motor control.

[7]  B. E. Maki,et al.  Change-in-support balance reactions in older persons: an emerging research area of clinical importance. , 2005, Neurologic clinics.

[8]  Daniel S Marigold,et al.  Whole-Body Responses: Neural Control and Implications for Rehabilitation and Fall Prevention , 2009, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[9]  Brian E. Maki,et al.  Resolving conflicts in task demands during balance recovery: does holding an object inhibit compensatory grasping? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  W. Gage,et al.  Parallels in control of voluntary and perturbation-evoked reach-to-grasp movements: EMG and kinematics , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  B. E. Maki,et al.  Early activation of arm muscles follows external perturbation of upright stance , 1995, Neuroscience Letters.

[12]  J. Allum,et al.  Influence of postural anxiety on postural reactions to multi-directional surface rotations. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  D. Westwood,et al.  No Evidence for Accurate Visuomotor Memory: Systematic and Variable Error in Memory-Guided Reaching , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.

[14]  W. McIlroy,et al.  Gaze behavior governing balance recovery in an unfamiliar and complex environment , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.

[15]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Vision and motor control , 1992 .

[16]  B. E. Maki,et al.  The role of limb movements in maintaining upright stance: the "change-in-support" strategy. , 1997, Physical therapy.

[17]  B. E. Maki,et al.  Does the "eyes lead the hand" principle apply to reach-to-grasp movements evoked by unexpected balance perturbations? , 2011, Human movement science.

[18]  Brian E. Maki,et al.  Initiation of rapid reach-and-grasp balance reactions: is a pre-formed visuospatial map used in controlling the initial arm trajectory? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  B. E. Maki,et al.  Effect of competing attentional demands on perturbation-evoked stepping reactions and associated gaze behavior in young and older adults. , 2008, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[20]  Gavin P. Lawrence,et al.  Inferring online and offline processing of visual feedback in target-directed movements from kinematic data , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[21]  Brian E. Maki,et al.  Efficacy of handrails in preventing stairway falls: a new experimental approach , 1998 .

[22]  B. E. Maki,et al.  The use of peripheral vision to guide perturbation-evoked reach-to-grasp balance-recovery reactions , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[24]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[25]  B. Maki,et al.  The moveable handhold: a new paradigm to study visual contributions to the control of balance-recovery reactions. , 2009, Gait & posture.

[26]  W. E. McIlroy,et al.  Changes in early ‘automatic’ postural responses associated with the prior-planning and execution of a compensatory step , 1993, Brain Research.

[27]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Influence of Premovement Visual Information on Manual Aiming , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[28]  M. Lemay,et al.  Effects of Target Presentation Time, Recall Delay, and Aging on the Accuracy of Manual Pointing to Remembered Targets , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  B. Maki,et al.  Stepping to recover balance in complex environments: Is online visual control of the foot motion necessary or sufficient? , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[30]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Goal-Directed Aiming: Correcting a Force-Specification Error With the Right and Left Hands. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[31]  B. E. Maki,et al.  Redirection of gaze and switching of attention during rapid stepping reactions evoked by unpredictable postural perturbation , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  M. Land Eye movements and the control of actions in everyday life , 2006, Progress in Retinal and Eye Research.

[33]  D. Ballard,et al.  Eye movements in natural behavior , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  Richard A. Abrams,et al.  Coordination of eye and hand for aimed limb movements. , 1992 .

[35]  Brian E. Maki,et al.  Gaze Behavior of Older Adults in Responding to Unexpected Loss of Balance while Walking in an Unfamiliar Environment: a Pilot Study , 2009 .

[36]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Integration of visual information and motor output in reaching and grasping: The contributions of peripheral and central vision , 1990, Neuropsychologia.