Attitude Importance and Resistance to Persuasion: It's Not Just the Thought That Counts

This research examined individual differences in attitude importance (J. A. Krosnick, 1988a) as a moderator of resistance to persuasion. In 2 studies, individuals who favored allowing gay people to serve openly in the military were aurally presented with a counterattitudinal message. Participants who considered their attitude high (vs. low) in personal importance were more resistant to the message. Process analyses revealed that both thought listings and self-reported affect mediated this attitude importance effect. A 2nd study, which also examined message quality, showed that both highand low-importance individuals were more resistant to a weak (vs. strong) message. This effect was explained by the fact that the weak (vs. strong) message engendered more irritation and negative affective elaborations. Results highlight the role of attitude importance in motivating resistance to persuasive communications and reveal that the resistance process is both cognitive and affective. Implications for contemporary models of persuasion are discussed.

[1]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Attitude strength, attitude structure, and resistance to change , 1995 .

[2]  Russell H. Fazio,et al.  Attitudes as object-evaluation associations: Determinants, consequences, and correlates of attitude accessibility. , 1995 .

[3]  G. Bodenhausen,et al.  Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sadness , 1994 .

[4]  R. Crutchfield,et al.  Theory and problems of social psychology , 1948 .

[5]  L. Festinger,et al.  A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance , 2017 .

[6]  J. Krosnick,et al.  Origins of attitude importance: self-interest, social identification, and value relevance. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  H. Laborit,et al.  [Experimental study]. , 1958, Bulletin mensuel - Societe de medecine militaire francaise.

[8]  Carl I. Hovland,et al.  Reconciling conflicting results derived from experimental and survey studies of attitude change. , 1959 .

[9]  Jon A. Krosnick,et al.  Attitude Importance and Attitude Accessibility , 1989 .

[10]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  Forewarning, cognitive responding, and resistance to persuasion. , 1977 .

[11]  J. Krosnick The role of attitude importance in social evaluation: a study of policy preferences, presidential candidate evaluations, and voting behavior. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  Leila T. Worth,et al.  Processing deficits and the mediation of positive affect in persuasion. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  J. Krosnick,et al.  The causes and consequences of attitude importance. , 1995 .

[15]  Larry E. Toothaker,et al.  Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions , 1991 .

[16]  Alice H. Eagly,et al.  Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. , 1989 .

[17]  D. Bem Self-Perception Theory , 1972 .

[18]  R. Abelson Beliefs Are Like Possessions , 1986 .

[19]  L. Festinger Conflict, Decision, and Dissonance , 1964 .

[20]  J. Brehm A theory of psychological reactance. , 1981 .

[21]  F. Heider The psychology of interpersonal relations , 1958 .

[22]  P. Tannenbaum,et al.  Theories of cognitive consistency: a sourcebook. , 1968 .

[23]  A. Elliot,et al.  Prejudice with and without compunction , 1991 .

[24]  R. Batra,et al.  Affective Responses Mediating Acceptance of Advertising , 1986 .

[25]  Muzafer Sherif,et al.  Attitude and Attitude Change: The Social Judgment-Involvement Approach , 1982 .

[26]  J. Krosnick Americans' Perceptions of Presidential Candidates: A Test of the Projection Hypothesis , 1990 .

[27]  J. A. Hardyck,et al.  Consistency, relevance, and resistance to change , 1966 .

[28]  J. Freedman,et al.  WARNING, DISTRACTION, AND RESISTANCE TO INFLUENCE. , 1965, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  Eugene Borgida,et al.  Personal involvement: An examination of processing differences. , 1986 .

[30]  Julie A. Edell,et al.  The Impact of Feelings on Ad-Based Affect and Cognition , 1989 .

[31]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.

[32]  Blair T. Johnson,et al.  Involvement and Persuasion: Types, Traditions, and the Evidence , 1990 .

[33]  Robert P. Abelson,et al.  Negative persuasion via personal insult , 1967 .

[34]  R. Petty,et al.  Attitude strength : antecedents and consequences , 1995 .

[35]  L. Ross,et al.  Biased Assimilation and Attitude Polarization: The Effects of Prior Theories on Subsequently Considered Evidence , 1979 .

[36]  Diane M. Mackie,et al.  Feeling Good, But Not Thinking Straight: The Impact of Positive Mood on Persuasion , 1991 .

[37]  I. Janis,et al.  An experimental study of psychological resistance to fear-arousing communications. , 1962, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[38]  P. Homer,et al.  Message Framing and the Interrelationships among Ad-Based Feelings, Affect, and Cognition , 1992 .

[39]  M. Zanna Message receptivity: A new look at the old problem of open- versus closed-mindedness. , 1993 .

[40]  D. Mackie,et al.  The social cognition analysis of social influence: Contributions to the understanding of persuasion and conformity. , 1994 .

[41]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Attitude strength and resistance processes. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[42]  C. Sherif,et al.  Personal involvement, social judgment, and action. , 1973 .

[43]  S. Chaiken The heuristic model of persuasion. , 1987 .

[44]  Shelly Chaiken,et al.  Beyond accuracy: Defense and impression motives in heuristic and systematic information processing. , 1996 .

[45]  S. Asch,et al.  The psychology of ego-involvements: Social attitudes and identifications. , 1948 .

[46]  A. A. Lumsdaine Communication and persuasion , 1954 .

[47]  A. Elliot,et al.  On the motivational nature of cognitive dissonance: Dissonance as psychological discomfort. , 1994 .

[48]  Jon A. Krosnick,et al.  Measures and manipulations of strength-related properties of attitudes: Current practice and future directions. , 1995 .

[49]  J. Krosnick,et al.  Attitude strength: One construct or many related constructs? , 1993 .

[50]  Ramon J. Rhine,et al.  Ego-involvement, discrepancy, source credibility, and attitude change. , 1970 .

[51]  D. Gilbert The assent of man: Mental representation and the control of belief. , 1993 .

[52]  Diane M. Mackie,et al.  Processing Rational and Emotional Messages: The Cognitive and Affective Mediation of Persuasion , 1995 .

[53]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  Estimating the effects of social interventions , 1981 .

[54]  Richard E. Petty,et al.  Multiple Roles for Affect in Persuasion , 1991 .

[55]  R. Petty,et al.  Positive Mood and Persuasion: Different Roles for Affect Under High- and Low-Elaboration Conditions , 1993 .

[56]  Franziska Marquart,et al.  Communication and persuasion : central and peripheral routes to attitude change , 1988 .

[57]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Mental contamination and mental correction: unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[58]  Robert P. Abelson,et al.  Modes of resolution of belief dilemmas , 1959 .

[59]  Leila T. Worth,et al.  Processing of persuasive in-group messages. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[60]  Norman Miller,et al.  Involvement and dogmatism as inhibitors of attitude change , 1965 .

[61]  Dieter Frey,et al.  Recent Research on Selective Exposure to Information , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.

[62]  D. Raden,et al.  Strength-related attitude dimensions , 1985 .

[63]  Paul D. Sweeney,et al.  Selective exposure: Voter information preferences and the Watergate affair. , 1984 .

[64]  M. Sherif,et al.  The psychology of attitudes. , 1946, Psychological review.

[65]  C. I. Hovland,et al.  Social Judgment: Assimilation and Contrast Effects in Communication and Attitude Change , 1981 .

[66]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[67]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  Effects of Forwarning of Persuasive Intent and Involvement on Cognitive Responses and Persuasion , 1979 .

[68]  Peter H. Ditto,et al.  Motivated Skepticism: Use of Differential Decision Criteria for Preferred and Nonpreferred Conclusions , 1992 .

[69]  B. J. Fine Conclusion-drawing, communicator credibility, and anxiety as factors in opinion change. , 1957, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[70]  G. Gorn The effects of personal involvement, communication discrepancy, and source prestige on reactions to communications on separatism , 1975 .

[71]  S. Sherman,et al.  Attitude bolstering when behavior is inconsistent with central attitudes , 1980 .

[72]  Blair T. Johnson,et al.  Effects of involvement on persuasion: a meta-analysis , 1989 .

[73]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Motivated resistance and openness to persuasion in the presence or absence of prior information. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[74]  N. Schwarz,et al.  Happy and Mindless, But Sad and Smart? The Impact of Affective States on Analytic Reasoning , 1991 .

[75]  R. Glen Hass,et al.  Temporal delay, type of forewarning, and resistance to influence , 1975 .

[76]  L. Doob Some factors determining change in attitude. , 1940 .

[77]  Richard Reardon,et al.  Forewarning of content and involvement: Consequences for persuasion and resistance to persuasion , 1992 .

[78]  L. Festinger,et al.  The effectiveness of unanticipated persuasive communications. , 1961, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[79]  J. Krosnick Attitude importance and attitude change , 1988 .