Detecting evolutionary forces in language change

Both language and genes evolve by transmission over generations with opportunity for differential replication of forms. The understanding that gene frequencies change at random by genetic drift, even in the absence of natural selection, was a seminal advance in evolutionary biology. Stochastic drift must also occur in language as a result of randomness in how linguistic forms are copied between speakers. Here we quantify the strength of selection relative to stochastic drift in language evolution. We use time series derived from large corpora of annotated texts dating from the 12th to 21st centuries to analyse three well-known grammatical changes in English: the regularization of past-tense verbs, the introduction of the periphrastic ‘do’, and variation in verbal negation. We reject stochastic drift in favour of selection in some cases but not in others. In particular, we infer selection towards the irregular forms of some past-tense verbs, which is likely driven by changing frequencies of rhyming patterns over time. We show that stochastic drift is stronger for rare words, which may explain why rare forms are more prone to replacement than common ones. This work provides a method for testing selective theories of language change against a null model and reveals an underappreciated role for stochasticity in language evolution.

[1]  Steven Pinker,et al.  Generalisation of regular and irregular morphological patterns , 1993 .

[2]  C. Condoravdi,et al.  Tracking Jespersen's Cycle , 2004 .

[3]  S. Shennan,et al.  A non-equilibrium neutral model for analysing cultural change. , 2013, Journal of theoretical biology.

[4]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  The evolution of frequency distributions: Relating regularization to inductive biases through iterated learning , 2009, Cognition.

[5]  W. Labov Principles of Linguistic Change; Volume 3; Cognitive and cultural factors , 2010 .

[6]  Ö. Dahl,et al.  Typology of sentence negation , 1979 .

[7]  Yuen Ren Chao,et al.  Human Behavior and the Principle of Least Effort: An Introduction to Human Ecology , 1950 .

[8]  J. Hawkins A parsing theory of word order universals , 1990 .

[9]  Morris Halle,et al.  The rules of language , 1980, IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication.

[10]  H. Hoenigswald THE ANNUS MIRABILIS 1876 AND POSTERITY , 1978 .

[11]  A. Ellegård The auxiliary do : the establishment and regulation of its use in English , 1955 .

[12]  M. Pagel,et al.  Frequency of word-use predicts rates of lexical evolution throughout Indo-European history , 2007, Nature.

[13]  H. Paul,et al.  Principles of the history of language , 1888 .

[14]  M. Kimura,et al.  An introduction to population genetics theory , 1971 .

[15]  J. Plotkin,et al.  Identifying Signatures of Selection in Genetic Time Series , 2013, Genetics.

[16]  T. M. Ellison,et al.  Cultural selection drives the evolution of human communication systems , 2014, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  Erez Lieberman,et al.  Quantifying the evolutionary dynamics of language , 2007, Nature.

[18]  M. Feldman,et al.  Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. , 1981, Monographs in population biology.

[19]  Erez Lieberman Aiden,et al.  Quantitative Analysis of Culture Using Millions of Digitized Books , 2010, Science.

[20]  M. Ullman Acceptability Ratings of Regular and Irregular Past-tense Forms: Evidence for a Dual-system Model of Language from Word Frequency and Phonological Neighbourhood Effects , 1999 .

[21]  S. Wright,et al.  Evolution in Mendelian Populations. , 1931, Genetics.

[22]  Nick Chater,et al.  Creating Language: Integrating Evolution, Acquisition, and Processing , 2016 .

[23]  Christopher Andrew Ahern,et al.  Cycles and Stability in Linguistic Signaling , 2016 .

[24]  D. Futuyma,et al.  The Princeton Guide to Evolution , 2013 .

[25]  P. Wallage Jespersen’s Cycle in Middle English: Parametric variation and grammatical competition , 2008 .

[26]  Slav Petrov,et al.  Syntactic Annotations for the Google Books NGram Corpus , 2012, ACL.

[27]  J. Sobel,et al.  STRATEGIC INFORMATION TRANSMISSION , 1982 .

[28]  Thomas L Griffiths,et al.  Words as alleles: connecting language evolution with Bayesian learners to models of genetic drift , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[29]  Scott A. Schwenter Fine-tuning Jespersen’s Cycle , 2006 .

[30]  Stephen J Shennan,et al.  Random drift and culture change , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[31]  P. Wallage Functional differentiation and grammatical competition in the English Jespersen Cycle , 2013 .

[32]  Marcus W. Feldman,et al.  Cultural Transmission and Evolution (MPB-16), Volume 16: A Quantitative Approach. (MPB-16) , 1981 .

[33]  George Kingsley Zipf,et al.  Human behavior and the principle of least effort , 1949 .

[34]  Ö. Dahl Inflationary effects in language and elsewhere , 2001 .

[35]  B. Joseph,et al.  Language History, Language Change, and Language Relationship: An Introduction to Historical and Comparative Linguistics , 1996 .

[36]  W. Strange Evolution of language. , 1984, JAMA.

[37]  W. Fitch,et al.  Linguistics: An invisible hand , 2007, Nature.

[38]  R. Jakobson On Language , 1990 .

[39]  W. Bruce Croft,et al.  Modeling language change: An evaluation of Trudgill's theory of the emergence of New Zealand English , 2009, Language Variation and Change.

[40]  O. Jespersen Negation in English and other languages , 1917 .

[41]  Matthew W. Hahn,et al.  Drift as a mechanism for cultural change: an example from baby names , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[42]  Joan L. Bybee,et al.  From Usage to Grammar: The Mind's Response to Repetition , 2007 .

[43]  R. Gray,et al.  Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo-European origin , 2003, Nature.

[44]  Carol Lynn Moder,et al.  Morphological Classes as Natural Categories , 1983 .

[45]  P. Pye-Smith The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex , 1871, Nature.

[46]  W. Labov,et al.  Empirical foundations for a theory of language change , 2014 .

[47]  Jean-Christophe Verstraete Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure , 2005 .

[48]  J. Harkins Review of Labov, William (1994) Principles of linguistic change, Volume 1: Internal factors , 1996 .

[49]  Johann Gustav Droysen Allgemeine Monatsschrift für Wissenschaft und Literatur , 1852 .

[50]  Richard A. William Blythe,et al.  S-curves and the mechanisms of propagation in language change , 2012 .

[51]  A. Kroch Reflexes of grammar in patterns of language change , 1989, Language Variation and Change.

[52]  Claudio Castellano,et al.  Internal and External Dynamics in Language: Evidence from Verb Regularity in a Historical Corpus of English , 2014, PloS one.

[53]  Mark Davies Expanding horizons in historical linguistics with the 400-million word Corpus of Historical American English , 2012 .

[54]  William S-Y. Wang Competing Changes as a Cause of Residue , 1969 .

[55]  H. H. Hock Principles of historical linguistics , 1986 .

[56]  M. Pagel Human language as a culturally transmitted replicator , 2009, Nature Reviews Genetics.

[57]  W. Kretzschmar THE PHILADELPHIA STORYPrinciples of Linguistic Change Volume 2, Social Factors , 2005 .

[58]  Richard A. Blythe,et al.  Neutral Evolution: a Null Model for Language Dynamics , 2011, Adv. Complex Syst..

[59]  Tandy Warnow,et al.  Indo‐European and Computational Cladistics , 2002 .