Words and pictures: An electrophysiological investigation of domain specific processing in native Chinese and English speakers
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] David L. Sheinberg,et al. The role of category learning in the acquisition and retention of perceptual expertise: A behavioral and neurophysiological study , 2008, Brain Research.
[2] Daniel Brandeis,et al. Evidence for developmental changes in the visual word processing network beyond adolescence , 2006, NeuroImage.
[3] Ying Liu,et al. ERP evidence for the time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic information in Chinese meaning and pronunciation decisions. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[4] H. Neville,et al. Maturational Constraints on Functional Specializations for Language Processing: ERP and Behavioral Evidence in Bilingual Speakers , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[5] J. Grainger,et al. Crowding affects letters and symbols differently. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] Denis Fize,et al. Speed of processing in the human visual system , 1996, Nature.
[7] Marcus Taft,et al. Positional Specificity of Radicals in Chinese Character Recognition , 1999 .
[8] Daniel Brandeis,et al. Coarse neural tuning for print peaks when children learn to read , 2006, NeuroImage.
[9] D Friedman,et al. Scaling is necessary when making comparisons between shapes of event-related potential topographies: a reply to Haig et al. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[10] James W. Tanaka,et al. A Reevaluation of the Electrophysiological Correlates of Expert Object Processing , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] C. C. Wood,et al. Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[12] M. Kutas,et al. Event-related brain potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words , 1980, Biological Psychology.
[13] J. Tanaka,et al. A Neural Basis for Expert Object Recognition , 2001, Psychological science.
[14] Blake W. Johnson,et al. Comparison of the N300 and N400 ERPs to picture stimuli in congruent and incongruent contexts , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[15] J. Kounios,et al. Dual-coding, context-availability, and concreteness effects in sentence comprehension: an electrophysiological investigation. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[16] C. Joyce,et al. The face-sensitive N170 and VPP components manifest the same brain processes: The effect of reference electrode site , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[17] J. Pernier,et al. ERP Manifestations of Processing Printed Words at Different Psycholinguistic Levels: Time Course and Scalp Distribution , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[18] M. Kutas,et al. Neurophysiological evidence for visual perceptual categorization of words and faces within 150 ms. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[19] H. Neville,et al. Language and , 2019, Adventure Diffusion.
[20] Bruno Rossion,et al. Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.
[21] E. Halgren,et al. Cognitive response profile of the human fusiform face area as determined by MEG. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.
[22] James W. Tanaka,et al. Activation of Preexisting and Acquired Face Representations: The N250 Event-related Potential as an Index of Face Familiarity , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[23] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area , 2004, NeuroImage.
[24] T. Allison,et al. Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe , 1994, Nature.
[25] M. Rugg,et al. Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of pictures , 1990, Brain and Cognition.
[26] J. Theios,et al. Theoretical analysis of the cognitive processing of lexical and pictorial stimuli: reading, naming, and visual and conceptual comparisons. , 1989, Psychological review.
[27] K R Paap,et al. Word shape's in poor shape for the race to the lexicon. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[28] J. G. Snodgrass,et al. A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.
[29] M. Potter,et al. Time to understand pictures and words , 1975, Nature.
[30] P. Holcomb,et al. Event-Related Brain Potentials Reflect Semantic Priming in an Object Decision Task , 1994, Brain and Cognition.
[31] T. Allison,et al. Electrophysiological Studies of Face Perception in Humans , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[32] P. Holcomb. Semantic priming and stimulus degradation: implications for the role of the N400 in language processing. , 2007, Psychophysiology.
[33] Marta Kutas,et al. Neurophysiological Evidence for the Time Course of Activation of Global Shape, Part, and Local Contour Representations during Visual Object Categorization and Memory , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[34] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Cracking the orthographic code: An introduction , 2008 .
[35] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Meaning and modality: influences of context, semantic memory organization, and perceptual predictability on picture processing. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[36] N. Fujimaki,et al. Masked immediate-repetition-priming effect on the early lexical process in the bilateral anterior temporal areas assessed by neuromagnetic responses , 2010, Neuroscience Research.
[37] R. Salmelin,et al. Dynamics of letter string perception in the human occipitotemporal cortex. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[38] Bruce D. McCandliss,et al. Fast, visual specialization for reading in English revealed by the topography of the N170 ERP response , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[39] Marta Kutas,et al. Time Course of Processes and Representations Supporting Visual Object Identification and Memory , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[40] Jonathan Grainger,et al. On the Time Course of Visual Word Recognition: An Event-related Potential Investigation using Masked Repetition Priming , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[41] Marta Kutas,et al. The intractability of scaling scalp distributions to infer neuroelectric sources. , 2002, Psychophysiology.
[42] Hyun Wook Park,et al. Spatiotemporal brain activation pattern during word/picture perception by native Koreans , 2004, Neuroreport.
[43] J. Grainger,et al. Exploring the temporal dynamics of visual word recognition in the masked repetition priming paradigm using event-related potentials , 2007, Brain Research.
[44] S. Geisser,et al. On methods in the analysis of profile data , 1959 .
[45] Marianna D. Eddy,et al. Masked repetition priming and event-related brain potentials: a new approach for tracking the time-course of object perception. , 2006, Psychophysiology.
[46] M. Bar,et al. Humans Prefer Curved Visual Objects , 2006, Psychological science.
[47] Urs Maurer,et al. Left-lateralized N170 Effects of Visual Expertise in Reading: Evidence from Japanese Syllabic and Logographic Scripts , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[48] C. C. Wood,et al. Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[49] P. Holcomb,et al. An electrophysiological investigation of semantic priming with pictures of real objects. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[50] M. Sigman,et al. The neural code for written words: a proposal , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[51] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Serial position effects in the identification of letters, digits, and symbols. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[52] J. Tanaka,et al. An electrophysiological comparison of visual categorization and recognition memory , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.