Putting motor resonance in perspective

Perceiving another person's actions changes the spatial perspective people use to describe objects in a scene, possibly because seeing human action induces people to map the actions, including their spatial context, to their own body and motor representations [Lozano, S. C., Hard, B. M., & Tversky, B. (2007). Putting action in perspective. Cognition 103, 480-490]. If so, then the effect of perceived action on perspective should be modulated by action experience. The present studies tested this prediction by showing participants a photograph and asking them to describe the location of one object relative to another. Across studies, adoption of the actor's perspective increased when participants saw actions that were more familiar or were performed with a limb (hand or foot) that observers had more experience performing actions with themselves. The present findings indicate that observers' experience performing observed actions influences their tendency to engage in self-other mapping, and consequently, their interpretations and descriptions of spatial scenes.

[1]  W. Carpenter Principles of Mental Physiology , 1874, Nature.

[2]  C. Darwin The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals , .

[3]  A. Glenberg,et al.  What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[4]  I. Johnsrude,et al.  Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[5]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  B. Tversky,et al.  Spatial mental models derived from survey and route descriptions , 1992 .

[7]  R. Johansson,et al.  Action plans used in action observation , 2003, Nature.

[8]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[9]  G. Bower,et al.  Mental models in narrative comprehension. , 1990, Science.

[10]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Visuospatial Thinking: Functional Significance of Visuospatial Representations , 2005 .

[11]  R. Passingham,et al.  Seeing or Doing? Influence of Visual and Motor Familiarity in Action Observation , 2006, Current Biology.

[12]  A M Liberman,et al.  Perception of the speech code. , 1967, Psychological review.

[13]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Perception of motion affects language processing , 2005, Cognition.

[14]  A. Glenberg,et al.  Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension , 1987 .

[15]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  Listening to Action-related Sentences Activates Fronto-parietal Motor Circuits , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  G. Lakoff,et al.  The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[17]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Grounding language in action , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[18]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  G. Knoblich,et al.  The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  K. Amunts,et al.  Broca's region subserves imagery of motion: A combined cytoarchitectonic and fMRI study , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[21]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[22]  M. Arbib,et al.  Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[23]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Body posture facilitates retrieval of autobiographical memories , 2007, Cognition.

[24]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[25]  B. Tversky,et al.  Searching imagined environments. , 1990 .

[26]  R. Klatzky The role of motor representations in semantic sensibility judgments , 1989 .

[27]  R. Hari,et al.  Temporal dynamics of cortical representation for action. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[29]  W. Prinz What Re-Enactment Earns Us , 2006, Cortex.

[30]  R. Gordon Folk Psychology as Simulation , 1986 .

[31]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: a TMS study , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.

[32]  L. Connell Representing object colour in language comprehension , 2007, Cognition.

[33]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[34]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[35]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  New aspects of motion perception: selective neural encoding of apparent human movements , 2000, Neuroreport.

[36]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Putting words in perspective , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[37]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[38]  Gian Luca Romani,et al.  Neural systems underlying observation of humanly impossible movements: an FMRI study. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  A. Clark Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  The psychology of the emotions , 2006 .

[41]  M. Jeannerod Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.

[42]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: with special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. , 1970, Psychological review.

[43]  R. E Passingham,et al.  Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the human brain: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[44]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  The Effect of Implied Orientation Derived from Verbal Context on Picture Recognition , 2001, Psychological science.

[45]  P. Harris Children and Emotion: The Development of Psychological Understanding , 1991 .

[46]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  Putting action in perspective , 2007, Cognition.

[47]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Language Comprehenders Mentally Represent the Shapes of Objects , 2002, Psychological science.

[48]  Jérôme Dokic,et al.  Simulation and Knowledge of Action , 2002 .

[49]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Moving words: dynamic representations in language comprehension , 2004 .

[50]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[51]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs , 2003, Cogn. Sci..

[52]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Weaving the fabric of social interaction: Articulating developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience in the domain of motor cognition , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[53]  G. V. Jones,et al.  Generalizing everyday memory: Signs and handedness , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[54]  E. Rosch,et al.  The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience , 1993 .

[55]  J. Baldwin Mental development in the child and the race , 2010 .

[56]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Functional links between motor and language systems , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[57]  Margaret Wilson,et al.  Six views of embodied cognition , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[58]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[59]  D C Rubin,et al.  A schema for common cents , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[60]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Involved in the Recognition of Actions Performed by Nonconspecifics: An fMRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt Human ethology: concepts and implications for the sciences of man , 1979 .