Perceptual consequences of interocular differences in the duration of temporal integration
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Johannes Burge,et al. Contact lenses, the reverse Pulfrich effect, and anti-Pulfrich monovision corrections , 2020, Scientific Reports.
[2] Johannes Burge,et al. Target tracking reveals the time course of visual processing with millisecond-scale precision , 2020, bioRxiv.
[3] Johannes Burge,et al. Image-Computable Ideal Observers for Tasks with Natural Stimuli. , 2020, Annual review of vision science.
[4] Johannes Burge,et al. Natural scene statistics predict how humans pool information across space in surface tilt estimation , 2019, bioRxiv.
[5] Seung Hyun Min,et al. Interocular Differences in Spatial Frequency Influence the Pulfrich Effect , 2020, Vision.
[6] Benjamin M. Chin,et al. Predicting the Partition of Behavioral Variability in Speed Perception with Naturalistic Stimuli , 2019, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[7] Johannes Burge,et al. Monovision and the Misperception of Motion , 2019, Current Biology.
[8] Alexander C. Huk,et al. Lawful tracking of visual motion in humans, macaques, and marmosets in a naturalistic, continuous, and untrained behavioral context , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[9] Johannes Burge,et al. The lawful imprecision of human surface tilt estimation in natural scenes , 2017, bioRxiv.
[10] R. Hess,et al. Interocular contrast difference drives illusory 3D percept , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[11] Kathryn Bonnen,et al. Dynamic mechanisms of visually guided 3D motion tracking. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.
[12] Julian Leyland,et al. The Southampton-York Natural Scenes (SYNS) dataset: Statistics of surface attitude , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[13] Brian C. McCann,et al. Estimating 3D tilt from local image cues in natural scenes , 2016, Journal of vision.
[14] Stephen G. Lisberger,et al. Signal, Noise, and Variation in Neural and Sensory-Motor Latency , 2016, Neuron.
[15] Wilson S. Geisler,et al. Optimal speed estimation in natural image movies predicts human performance , 2015, Nature Communications.
[16] Johannes Burge,et al. Continuous psychophysics: Target-tracking to measure visual sensitivity. , 2015, Journal of vision.
[17] W. Geisler,et al. Optimal disparity estimation in natural stereo images. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[18] Lawrence K. Cormack,et al. Reflexive and voluntary control of smooth eye movements , 2013, Electronic Imaging.
[19] Wilson S. Geisler,et al. Optimal defocus estimates from individual images for autofocusing a digital camera , 2012, Electronic Imaging.
[20] Johannes Burge,et al. Optimal defocus estimation in individual natural images , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[21] Geraint Rees,et al. Knowing with Which Eye We See: Utrocular Discrimination and Eye-Specific Signals in Human Visual Cortex , 2010, PloS one.
[22] W. Bialek,et al. A sensory source for motor variation , 2005, Nature.
[23] Bruce G Cumming,et al. Effect of interocular delay on disparity-selective v1 neurons: relationship to stereoacuity and the pulfrich effect. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[24] J. Movshon,et al. Adaptive Temporal Integration of Motion in Direction-Selective Neurons in Macaque Visual Cortex , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[25] Robert A. Frazor,et al. Visual cortex neurons of monkeys and cats: temporal dynamics of the spatial frequency response function. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[26] Dale Purves,et al. Image/source statistics of surfaces in natural scenes , 2003, Network.
[27] A. Vassilev,et al. On the delay in processing high spatial frequency visual information: reaction time and VEP latency study of the effect of local intensity of stimulation , 2002, Vision Research.
[28] James A. Crowell,et al. Horizontal and vertical disparity, eye position, and stereoscopic slant perception , 1999, Vision Research.
[29] D. Wolpert,et al. Retinal adaptation of visual processing time delays , 1993, Vision Research.
[30] B. Pesta,et al. A generalized visual latency explanation of the Pulfrich phenomenon , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[31] D. Levi,et al. Suprathreshold spatial frequency detection and binocular interaction in strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia. , 1979, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.
[32] S. Anstis,et al. Visual delay as a function of luminance. , 1969, The American journal of psychology.
[33] G. S. Harker,et al. Some observations and measurements of the Pulfrich phenomenon , 1967 .
[34] R. Weale. Theory of the Pulfrich effect. , 1954, Ophthalmologica. Journal international d'ophtalmologie. International journal of ophthalmology. Zeitschrift fur Augenheilkunde.
[35] A. Lit. The magnitude of the Pulfrich stereophenomenon as a function of binocular differences of intensity at various levels of illumination. , 1949, The American journal of psychology.
[36] C. Pulfrich. Die Stereoskopie im Dienste der isochromen und heterochromen Photometrie , 2005, Naturwissenschaften.
[37] D. Trincker. Hell-Dunkel-Anpassung und räumliches Sehen , 2004, Pflüger's Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.
[38] Martin S. Banks,et al. Extra-retinal and perspective cues cause the small range of the induced effect , 1998, Vision Research.
[39] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[40] Randolph Blake,et al. On utrocular discrimination , 1979 .
[41] S. Freguia. Researches in Binocular Vision. , 1950 .