Physics in the primary school: Peer interaction and the understanding of floating and sinking

[1]  C. Rodgers Maternal support for the Down's syndrome stereotype: the effect of direct experience of the condition. , 2008, Journal of mental deficiency research.

[2]  James V. Wertsch,et al.  Culture, communication, and cognition : Vygotskian perspectives , 1987 .

[3]  Organisation du produit de deux ensembles: Influence des interactions sociales entre pairs sur les procédures de résolution et les performances individuelles , 1987 .

[4]  G. Halford,et al.  Children's Concepts of Volume and Flotation. , 1986 .

[5]  E. Forman,et al.  The social origins of logic: the contributions of Piaget and Vygotsky. , 1985, New directions for child development.

[6]  D. Bearison,et al.  Social interaction, social observation, and cognitive development in young children , 1985 .

[7]  R. Driver,et al.  Children's Ideas in Science , 1985 .

[8]  Roger Osborne,et al.  Learning in science , 1985 .

[9]  C. Howe,et al.  The Development of Children's Social Rule Awareness through Cognitive Conflict and Social Interaction. , 1984 .

[10]  L. McDermott Research on conceptual understanding in mechanics , 1984 .

[11]  M. Gilly,et al.  Efficacité comparée du travail individuel et du travail en interaction socio-cognitive dans l'appropriation et la mise en œuvre de règles de résolution chez des enfants de 11-12 ans , 1984 .

[12]  W. Doise The social development of the intellect , 1984 .

[13]  Robert J. Whitaker,et al.  Aristotle is not dead: Student understanding of trajectory motion , 1983 .

[14]  R. Driver,et al.  Theories-in-Action: Some Theoretical and Empirical Issues in the Study of Students' Conceptual Frameworks in Science , 1983 .

[15]  John K. Gilbert,et al.  Concepts, Misconceptions and Alternative Conceptions: Changing Perspectives in Science Education , 1983 .

[16]  F. Murray,et al.  When two wrongs make a right: Promoting cognitive change by social conflict. , 1982 .

[17]  Melanie Killen,et al.  Peer interaction and the process of change in children's moral reasoning. , 1982 .

[18]  James A. Russell Cognitive Conflict, Transmission, and Justification: Conservation Attainment through Dyadic Interaction , 1982 .

[19]  L. E. Klopfer,et al.  Cognitive research and the design of science instruction , 1982 .

[20]  M. McCloskey Naive Theories of Motion. , 1982 .

[21]  J. Clement Students’ preconceptions in introductory mechanics , 1982 .

[22]  James A. Russell,et al.  Why ‘Socio‐cognitive Conflict’ may be Impossible: the status of egocentric errors in the dyadic performance of a spatial task , 1981 .

[23]  L. E. Klopfer,et al.  Factors influencing the learning of classical mechanics , 1980 .

[24]  Marvin W. Berkowitz,et al.  THE RELATION OF MORAL JUDGMENT STAGE DISPARITY TO DEVELOPMENTAL EFFECTS OF PEER DIALOGUES , 1980 .

[25]  E. Lunzer,et al.  The Effective Use of Reading. , 1980 .

[26]  Should science be taught in primary schools , 1980 .

[27]  Joseph D. Novak,et al.  Notions of Physical Laws in Childhood. , 1980 .

[28]  Gabriel Mugny,et al.  Individual and collective conflicts of centrations in cognitive development , 1979 .

[29]  W. Doise,et al.  Socio-cognitive conflict and structure of individual and collective performances , 1978 .

[30]  J. Piaget,et al.  The Moral Judgement of the Child , 1977 .

[31]  Maurice Galton,et al.  Processes and products of science teaching , 1976 .

[32]  J. Piaget,et al.  The principles of genetic epistemology , 1972 .

[33]  S. Engel,et al.  Thought and Language , 1964, Dialogue.

[34]  J. Piaget The Growth Of Logical Thinking From Childhood To Adolescence: An Essay On The Construction Of Formal Operational Structures , 1958 .

[35]  W. Richmond Education in England , 1945 .

[36]  J. Piaget The Child's Conception of Physical Causality , 1927 .