Nostril-Specific Olfactory Modulation of Visual Perception in Binocular Rivalry

It is known that olfaction and vision can work in tandem to represent object identities. What is yet unclear is the stage of the sensory processing hierarchy at which the two types of inputs converge. Here we study this issue through a well established visual phenomenon termed binocular rivalry. We show that smelling an odor from one nostril significantly enhances the dominance time of the congruent visual image in the contralateral visual field, relative to that in the ipsilateral visual field. Moreover, such lateralization-based enhancement extends to category selective regions so that when two images of words and human body, respectively, are engaged in rivalry in the central visual field, smelling natural human body odor from the right nostril increases the dominance time of the body image compared with smelling it from the left nostril. Semantic congruency alone failed to produce this effect in a similar setting. These results, taking advantage of the anatomical and functional lateralizations in the olfactory and visual systems, highlight the functional dissociation of the two nostrils and provide strong evidence for an object-based early convergence of olfactory and visual inputs in sensory representations.

[1]  Jessica Porter,et al.  Mechanisms of scent-tracking in humans , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  C. Wysocki,et al.  Nasal trigeminal chemosensitivity across the adult life span , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  Giuliano Iurilli,et al.  Sound-Driven Synaptic Inhibition in Primary Visual Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[4]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  Ankoor S. Shah,et al.  Auditory Cortical Neurons Respond to Somatosensory Stimulation , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Denise Chen,et al.  Binaral Rivalry between the Nostrils and in the Cortex , 2009, Current Biology.

[7]  Rebecca F. Schwarzlose,et al.  Separate face and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus. , 2010, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  Wen Zhou,et al.  Fear-Related Chemosignals Modulate Recognition of Fear in Ambiguous Facial Expressions , 2009, Psychological science.

[9]  G Raisman,et al.  The central olfactory connexions. , 1965, Journal of anatomy.

[10]  T. Stanford,et al.  Multisensory integration: current issues from the perspective of the single neuron , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  Thomas D Albright,et al.  On the Perception of Probable Things: Neural Substrates of Associative Memory, Imagery, and Perception , 2012, Neuron.

[12]  E. Macaluso,et al.  Multisensory spatial interactions: a window onto functional integration in the human brain , 2005, Trends in Neurosciences.

[13]  Johan N Lundström,et al.  Modulation of Olfactory Perception by Visual Cortex Stimulation , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  H. Kennedy,et al.  Anatomical Evidence of Multimodal Integration in Primate Striate Cortex , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  J. Pettigrew,et al.  Perceptual Rivalry as an Ultradian Oscillation , 2003 .

[16]  M. Herbin,et al.  Neuroanatomical pathways linking vision and olfaction in mammals , 1994, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[17]  J. Price An autoradiographic study of complementary laminar patterns of termination of afferent fibers to the olfactory cortex , 1973, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[18]  Michael S Beauchamp,et al.  See me, hear me, touch me: multisensory integration in lateral occipital-temporal cortex , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[19]  M. Morrone,et al.  Touch disambiguates rivalrous perception at early stages of visual analysis , 2010, Current Biology.

[20]  M. Wallace,et al.  A revised view of sensory cortical parcellation , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  D. Dubourdieu,et al.  The Color of Odors , 2001, Brain and Language.

[22]  M. Magnin,et al.  Retinal projection to the olfactory tubercle and basal telencephalon in primates , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[23]  J. Hellige Unity of Thought and Action: Varieties of Interaction Between the Left and Right Cerebral Hemispheres , 1993 .

[24]  R W Cox,et al.  Language processing is strongly left lateralized in both sexes. Evidence from functional MRI. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  David Alais,et al.  Multisensory Congruency as a Mechanism for Attentional Control over Perceptual Selection , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Roel M. Willems,et al.  Cerebral lateralization of face-selective and body-selective visual areas depends on handedness. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  G. E. Pickard,et al.  Direct retinal projections to the hypothalamus, piriform cortex, and accessory optic nuclei in the golden hamster as demonstrated by a sensitive anterograde horseradish peroxidase technique , 1981, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[28]  Scott D. Slotnick,et al.  The Visual Word Form Area , 2013 .

[29]  Denise Chen,et al.  Olfaction Modulates Visual Perception in Binocular Rivalry , 2010, Current Biology.

[30]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  R. Dolan,et al.  The Nose Smells What the Eye Sees Crossmodal Visual Facilitation of Human Olfactory Perception , 2003, Neuron.

[32]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Human Body , 2001 .