Electrophysiological correlates of perceptual reversals for three different types of multistable images.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] G. V. Simpson,et al. Flow of activation from V1 to frontal cortex in humans , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[2] David L. Sheinberg,et al. The role of temporal cortical areas in perceptual organization. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[3] G. Mangun. Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. , 1995, Psychophysiology.
[4] Michael A. Pitts,et al. Early top-down influences on bistable perception revealed by event-related potentials , 2008, Brain and Cognition.
[5] S. Hillyard,et al. Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[6] N. Logothetis,et al. Multistable phenomena: changing views in perception , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[7] Michael Bach,et al. Early neural activity in Necker-cube reversal: evidence for low-level processing of a gestalt phenomenon. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[8] D. Strüber,et al. Differences in Top—Down Influences on the Reversal Rate of Different Categories of Reversible Figures , 1999, Perception.
[9] E Başar,et al. Multistable visual perception induces a slow positive EEG wave. , 1993, The International journal of neuroscience.
[10] F. Tong,et al. Can attention selectively bias bistable perception? Differences between binocular rivalry and ambiguous figures. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[11] H G Smid,et al. The organization of multidimensional selection on the basis of color and shape: An event-related brain potential study , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[12] J. Orbach,et al. Reversibility of the Necker Cube: I. An Examination of the Concept of “Satiation of Orientation” , 1963, Perceptual and motor skills.
[13] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Reversible-figure perception: Mechanisms of intentional control , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[14] Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al. Localizing visual discrimination processes in time and space. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.
[15] F. Karayanidis,et al. An exploration of varieties of visual attention: ERP findings. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[16] G. Mangun,et al. Luminance and spatial attention effects on early visual processing. , 1995, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[17] Steven A. Hillyard,et al. Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: Psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. , 1994 .
[18] Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al. Human brain activity during spontaneously reversing perception of ambiguous figures , 1998, 5th IEEE EMBS International Summer School on Biomedical Imaging, 2002..
[19] S. Hillyard,et al. Spatial Selective Attention Affects Early Extrastriate But Not Striate Components of the Visual Evoked Potential , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] John Harris,et al. Biasing Effects in Ambiguous Figures: Removal or Fixation of Critical Features Can Affect Perception , 1997 .
[21] Richard B Buxton,et al. Putting spatial attention on the map: timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas , 2001, Vision Research.
[22] David A. Leopold,et al. Stable perception of visually ambiguous patterns , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[23] N. Squires,et al. Visual evoked potentials to illusory reversals of the necker cube. , 1988, Psychophysiology.
[24] N. Logothetis,et al. Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception. , 1989, Science.
[25] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Enduring interest in perceptual ambiguity: alternating views of reversible figures. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.
[26] C. Blundo,et al. Perception of ambiguous figures after focal brain lesions , 1990, Neuropsychologia.
[27] Toshio Inui,et al. Neural substrates for depth perception of the Necker cube; a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in human subjects , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.
[28] G. Shulman. Attentional effects on Necker cube adaptation , 1993 .
[29] N. Logothetis,et al. Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys' percepts during binocular rivalry , 1996, Nature.
[30] G. J. Brouwer,et al. Voluntary control and the dynamics of perceptual bi-stability , 2005, Vision Research.
[31] S. Luck. An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique , 2005 .
[32] S. Yantis,et al. Common neural substrates for the control and effects of visual attention and perceptual bistability. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[33] Sabine Windmann,et al. Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Attentional Control over Bistable Vision , 2006 .
[34] L. Cohen,et al. Rate of Apparent Change of a Necker Cube as a Function of Prior Stimulation , 1959 .
[35] Laurie A. Miller,et al. Perceptual flexibility after frontal or temporal lobectomy , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[36] M. Valdés-Sosa,et al. Switching Attention without Shifting the Spotlight: Object-Based Attentional Modulation of Brain Potentials , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[37] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Selective adaptation with reversible figures: Don’t change that channel , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.
[38] Ahmet Ademoglu,et al. Analysis of the electroencephalographic activity during the Necker cube reversals by means of the wavelet transform , 1998, Biological Cybernetics.
[39] W. Köhler. Dynamics in Psychology , 1940 .
[40] R. Liebert,et al. Voluntary Control of Reversible Figures , 1985, Perceptual and motor skills.
[41] H. Heinze,et al. An event-related brain potential study of visual selective attention to conjunctions of color and shape. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[42] T W Picton,et al. The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.
[43] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Prime time: Fatigue and set effects in the perception of reversible figures , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[44] S. Hillyard,et al. Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential , 2002, Human brain mapping.
[45] L. H. Pelton,et al. Acceleration of reversals of a Necker cube. , 1968, The American journal of psychology.
[46] Insights from event-related potentials into the temporal and hierarchical organization of the ventral and dorsal streams of the visual system in selective attention. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[47] I Rock,et al. Why do ambiguous figures reverse? , 1994, Acta psychologica.
[48] S. Luck,et al. Spatio‐temporal dynamics of attention to color: Evidence from human electrophysiology , 1998, Human brain mapping.
[49] S. Luck,et al. Sources of attention-sensitive visual event-related potentials , 2005, Brain Topography.
[50] N. Kawabata. Attention and Depth Perception , 1986, Perception.
[51] E. Vogel,et al. Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[52] Michael Bach,et al. The Necker cube—an ambiguous figure disambiguated in early visual processing , 2005, Vision Research.
[53] Raymond van Ee,et al. Dynamics of perceptual bi-stability for stereoscopic slant rivalry and a comparison with grating, house-face, and Necker cube rivalry , 2005, Vision Research.
[54] Krista L. Horlitz,et al. Satiation or availability? Effects of attention, memory, and imagery on the perception of ambiguous figures , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[55] S. Hillyard,et al. Selective attention to the color and direction of moving stimuli: Electrophysiological correlates of hierarchical feature selection , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[56] David A Leopold,et al. Visual Perception: Shaping What We See , 2003, Current Biology.
[57] P. Fox,et al. Retinotopic organization of early visual spatial attention effects as revealed by PET and ERPs , 1997, Human brain mapping.