Interference resolution in major depression

In two experiments, we investigated individual differences in the ability to resolve interference in participants diagnosed with major depressive disorder (MDD). Participants were administered the “Ignore/Suppress” task, a short-term memory task composed of two steps. In Step 1 (“ignore”), participants were instructed to memorize a set of stimuli while ignoring simultaneously presented irrelevant material. In Step 2 (“suppress”), participants were instructed to forget a subset of the previously memorized material. The ability to resolve interference was indexed by response latencies on two recognition tasks in which participants decided whether a probe was a member of the target set. In Step 1, we compared response latencies to probes from the to-be-ignored list with response latencies to nonrecently presented items. In Step 2, we compared response latencies to probes from the to-be-suppressed list with response latencies to nonrecently presented items. The results indicate that, compared with control participants, depressed participants exhibited increased interference in the “suppress” but not in the “ignore” step of the task, when the stimuli were negative words. No group differences were obtained when we presented letters instead of emotional words. These findings indicate that depression is associated with difficulty in removing irrelevant negative material from short-term memory.

[1]  Dirk Wentura,et al.  Activation and Inhibition of Affective Information: for Negative Priming in the Evaluation Task , 1999 .

[2]  D. Barch,et al.  Working memory and prefrontal cortex dysfunction: specificity to schizophrenia compared with major depression , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[3]  A. Miyake,et al.  Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control , 1999 .

[4]  P. Hertel Relation between rumination and impaired memory in dysphoric moods. , 1998, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[5]  N. Raoux,et al.  Executive functioning and verbal memory in young patients with unipolar depression and schizophrenia , 1999, Psychiatry Research.

[6]  Richard L. Lewis,et al.  The mind and brain of short-term memory. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[7]  M. Robertson,et al.  Working memory in clinical depression: an experimental study , 1993, Psychological Medicine.

[8]  A. Miyake,et al.  The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[9]  R. de Raedt,et al.  Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression. , 2006, Journal of affective disorders.

[10]  P. Linville,et al.  Attention inhibition: Does it underlie ruminative thought? , 1996 .

[11]  P. Hertel Memory for Emotional and Nonemotional Events in Depression: A Question of Habit? , 2004 .

[12]  P. Hertel On the contribution of deficient cognitive control to memory impairment in depression , 1997 .

[13]  S. Nolen-Hoeksema,et al.  Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  M. Banich,et al.  Inhibition Versus Switching Deficits in Different Forms of Rumination , 2007, Psychological science.

[15]  Marc G. Berman,et al.  Neural mechanisms of proactive interference-resolution , 2007, NeuroImage.

[16]  A. Isen,et al.  Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. , 1984 .

[17]  J. Jonides,et al.  Assessing dysfunction using refined cognitive methods. , 2005, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[18]  S. Nolen-Hoeksema,et al.  Rumination Reconsidered: A Psychometric Analysis , 2003, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[19]  S. Tipper Does Negative Priming Reflect Inhibitory Mechanisms? A Review and Integration of Conflicting Views , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  M. Siemer Mood-congruent cognitions constitute mood experience. , 2005, Emotion.

[21]  I. Skre,et al.  High interrater reliability for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM‐III‐R Axis I (SCID‐I) , 1991, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[22]  S. Nolen-Hoeksema,et al.  A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: the 1989 Loma Prieta Earthquake. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  H. Ellis,et al.  Emotional mood states and memory: elaborative encoding, semantic processing, and cognitive effort. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  Jutta Joormann,et al.  Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibitory processes , 2004 .

[25]  H. Ellis,et al.  Irrelevant thoughts, emotional mood states, and cognitive task performance , 1991, Memory & cognition.

[26]  Jutta Joormann,et al.  Inhibition, Rumination, and Mood Regulation in Depression. , 2005 .

[27]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A Review and a New View , 1988 .

[28]  P. Maruff,et al.  Neuropsychological function in young patients with unipolar major depression , 1997, Psychological Medicine.

[29]  J. Jonides,et al.  Dissociable Interference-Control Processes in Perception and Memory , 2008, Psychological science.

[30]  M Davies,et al.  The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID). II. Multisite test-retest reliability. , 1992 .

[31]  Jutta Joormann,et al.  Updating the contents of working memory in depression: interference from irrelevant negative material. , 2008, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[32]  L. Alloy,et al.  The response styles theory of depression: tests and an extension of the theory. , 1997, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[33]  A. Beech,et al.  Obsessional states: anxiety disorders or schizotypes? An information processing and personality assessment , 1990, Psychological Medicine.

[34]  C. Frith,et al.  Consciousness, Information Processing and Schizophrenia , 1979, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[35]  J. Joormann Differential Effects of Rumination and Dysphoria on the Inhibition of Irrelevant Emotional Material: Evidence from a Negative Priming Task , 2006, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[36]  L Hasher,et al.  Age and inhibition. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[37]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings , 1999 .

[38]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Attention profile in schizophrenia compared with depression: differential effects of processing speed, selective attention and vigilance , 2003, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[39]  N. Cowan An embedded-processes model of working memory , 1999 .

[40]  R. Engle,et al.  Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. , 1999 .

[41]  A. Beck,et al.  Beck Depression Inventory–II , 2011 .

[42]  D. Balota,et al.  Individual differences in information-processing rate and amount: implications for group differences in response latency. , 1999, Psychological bulletin.

[43]  R. N. Davis,et al.  Cognitive Inflexibility Among Ruminators and Nonruminators , 2000, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[44]  S. Nolen-Hoeksema,et al.  The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. , 2000, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[45]  B. Fredrickson,et al.  Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. , 1993, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[46]  R. Erber,et al.  Beyond mood and social judgment: Mood incongruent recall and mood regulation , 1994 .

[47]  S. Lyubomirsky,et al.  Rethinking Rumination , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[48]  Klaus P. Ebmeier,et al.  Pattern of impaired working memory during major depression. , 2006, Journal of affective disorders.

[49]  C. L. Rusting,et al.  Retrieving positive memories to regulate negative mood: consequences for mood-congruent memory. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[50]  M. Thase,et al.  Cognitive disturbance in outpatient depressed younger adults: evidence of modest impairment , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[51]  R. Brown,et al.  Rumination and executive function in depression: an experimental study , 2002, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[52]  John Sabini,et al.  Mood and memory under natural conditions: Evidence for mood incongruent recall. , 1990 .

[53]  Cynthia P. May,et al.  Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. , 1999 .

[54]  S. Lyubomirsky,et al.  Self-perpetuating properties of dysphoric rumination. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[55]  Jutta Joormann,et al.  Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination? Diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases. , 2006, Behavior therapy.

[56]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  The directed forgetting task: application to emotionally valent material. , 2000, Journal of affective disorders.

[57]  B. Knowlton,et al.  Directed forgetting of emotional words. , 2008, Emotion.

[58]  B. Dubois,et al.  Executive functions and updating of the contents of working memory in unipolar depression. , 2004, Journal of psychiatric research.

[59]  M. First,et al.  Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders : SCID-I : clinical version : scoresheet , 1997 .

[60]  David F. Bjorklund,et al.  The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. , 1990 .