Revisiting the innate preference for consonance.

The origin of the Western preference for consonance remains unresolved, with some suggesting that the preference is innate. In Experiments 1 and 2 of the present study, 6-month-old infants heard six different consonant/dissonant pairs of stimuli, including those tested in previous research. In contrast to the findings of others, infants in the present study failed to listen longer to consonant stimuli. After 3 minutes of exposure to consonant or dissonant stimuli in Experiment 3, 6-month-old infants listened longer to the familiar stimulus, whether consonant or dissonant. Our findings are inconsistent with innate preferences for consonant stimuli. Instead, the effect of short-term exposure is consistent with the view that familiarity underlies the origin of the Western preference for consonant intervals.

[1]  Michael W. Weiss,et al.  Something in the Way She Sings , 2012, Psychological science.

[2]  R. G. Crowder,et al.  Perception of the major/minor distinction: V. Preferences among infants , 1991 .

[3]  J. Kagan,et al.  INFANTS' PERCEPTION OF CONSONANCE AND DISSONANCE IN MUSIC , 1998 .

[4]  R. Plomp,et al.  Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth. , 1965, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  Austin M. Brues,et al.  The Fusion of Non-Musical Intervals , 1927 .

[6]  P. Daston,et al.  Musical consonance as musical preference: a cross-cultural study. , 1968, The Journal of general psychology.

[7]  A. Kameoka,et al.  Consonance theory part I: consonance of dyads. , 1969, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  The impact of stimulus 'value' in infant novelty preference. , 2000, Developmental psychobiology.

[9]  E. G. Bugg An analysis of conditions influencing consonance discrimination. , 1939 .

[10]  Steven Brown,et al.  Universals in the world’s musics , 2013 .

[11]  B. Delgutte,et al.  Neurobiological Foundations for the Theory of Harmony in Western Tonal Music , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Catherine J. Stevens,et al.  Judgments of complexity and pleasingness in music: The effect of structure, Repetition, and training , 1991 .

[13]  S. Trehub,et al.  Key membership and implied harmony in Western tonal music: Developmental perspectives , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[14]  S. Trehub,et al.  Tuning in to musical rhythms: infants learn more readily than adults. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[15]  J. Richard,et al.  Attracting and Maintaining Infant Attention during Habituation: Further Evidence of the Importance of Stimulus Complexity. , 2004 .

[16]  Martha Guernsey,et al.  The Role of Consonance and Dissonance in Music , 1928 .

[17]  H. Helmholtz,et al.  On the Sensations of Tone as a Physiological Basis for the Theory of Music , 2005 .

[18]  Ernst Terhardt,et al.  The Concept of Musical Consonance: A Link between Music and Psychoacoustics , 1984 .

[19]  Constantine Frithiof Malmberg,et al.  The perception of consonance and dissonance , 1918 .

[20]  L. Trainor,et al.  Feeling the Beat: Movement Influences Infant Rhythm Perception , 2005, Science.

[21]  D. Oller,et al.  Innateness, Experience, and Music Perception , 1990 .

[22]  S. Trehub,et al.  Metrical Categories in Infancy and Adulthood , 2005, Psychological science.

[23]  E. Terhardt Psychoacoustic evaluation of musical sounds , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  Norman Cazden The Definition of Consonance and Dissonance , 1980 .

[25]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Natural Musical Intervals: Evidence From Infant Listeners , 1996 .

[26]  Laurel J. Trainor,et al.  Preference for Sensory Consonance in 2- and 4-Month-Old Infants , 2002 .

[27]  E. M. Burns Intervals, Scales, and Tuning , 1999 .

[28]  H. E. Krugman Affective response to music as a function of familiarity. , 1943 .

[29]  M. Spence Young infants' long-term auditory memory: evidence for changes in preference as a function of delay. , 1996, Developmental psychobiology.

[30]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Sensory consonance and the perceptual similarity of complex-tone harmonic intervals: tests of adult and infant listeners. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  L. Trainor,et al.  Long-term memory for music: infants remember tempo and timbre. , 2004, Developmental science.

[32]  James Tenney,et al.  A history of consonance and dissonance , 1988 .

[33]  A. Kameoka,et al.  Consonance theory part II: consonance of complex tones and its calculation method. , 1969, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[34]  R. Plomp,et al.  The connotation of musical consonance , 1962 .

[35]  Sarah J Wilson,et al.  Consonance and pitch. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[36]  Marilyn L. Shaw,et al.  Perceived Structure of Triads , 1984 .

[37]  Satsuki Nakai,et al.  Distinguishing novelty and familiarity effects in infant preference procedures , 2004 .

[38]  E. Terhardt Pitch, consonance, and harmony. , 1974, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[39]  D. Hargreaves,et al.  Musical Preference, Identification, and Familiarity , 2008 .

[40]  Sandra E. Trehub,et al.  Cross-cultural differences in meter perception , 2013, Psychological research.

[41]  Behavioral methods in infancy: pitfalls of single measures , 2012, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[42]  J. M. Hunt Attentional Preference and Experience: I. Introduction , 1970 .

[43]  T. F. Maher "Need for Resolution" Ratings for Harmonic Musical Intervals , 1976 .

[44]  N. Schwarz,et al.  Processing Fluency and Aesthetic Pleasure: Is Beauty in the Perceiver's Processing Experience? , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[45]  Ian L. Bradley,et al.  Repetition as a Factor in the Development of Musical Preferences , 1971 .

[46]  P. Vassilakis,et al.  Auditory roughness as means of musical expression , 2005 .

[47]  L. Trainor,et al.  The development of evaluative responses to music:: Infants prefer to listen to consonance over dissonance , 1998 .

[48]  Bruno Nettl An ethnomusicologist contemplates universals in musical sound and musical culture , 2000 .

[49]  P. Silvia What is interesting? Exploring the appraisal structure of interest. , 2005, Emotion.

[50]  C. Victor Fung,et al.  Musicians' and Nonmusicians' Preferences for World Musics: Relation to Musical Characteristics and Familiarity , 1996 .

[51]  S. Trehub,et al.  A comparison of infants' and adults' sensitivity to western musical structure. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  Josh H. McDermott,et al.  The basis of musical consonance as revealed by congenital amusia , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[53]  Nobuo Masataka,et al.  Preference for consonance over dissonance by hearing newborns of deaf parents and of hearing parents. , 2006, Developmental science.

[54]  Steven Brown Joseph Jordania. Who Asked the First Question? The Origin of Human Choral Singing, Intelligence, Language and Speech , 2009 .

[55]  L. Trainor,et al.  Hearing what the body feels: Auditory encoding of rhythmic movement , 2007, Cognition.

[56]  E. W. Ames,et al.  A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. , 1988 .

[57]  L. Robert Slevc,et al.  Music and Early Language Acquisition , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[58]  E. Brattico,et al.  Music and Emotions in the Brain: Familiarity Matters , 2011, PloS one.

[59]  Adrian C. North,et al.  Subjective complexity, familiarity, and liking for popular music. , 1995 .

[60]  Hermann L. F. Helmholtz,et al.  The sensations of tone: As a physiological basis for the theory of music (6th ed.). , 1948 .

[61]  Josh H. McDermott,et al.  Individual Differences Reveal the Basis of Consonance , 2010, Current Biology.