Forgetting no-longer-relevant prospective memory intentions is (sometimes) harder with age but easier with forgetting practice.

In young adults, it has been shown that prospective memory (PM) commission errors, the erroneous performance of a previously relevant intention, are less likely for repeatedly performed intentions (than never performed intentions). We examined whether this pattern holds for older adults, for whom impaired response inhibition processes might heighten risk of commission errors for repeatedly performed PM intentions. Older adults encoded a PM intention to press a key when a target word appeared during an ongoing lexical decision task. Target words were presented 4 (repeatedly) or 0 times before participants were instructed the PM task was finished and should not be performed again. Target words were then (re)presented and commission errors were recorded. Experiment 1 demonstrated it was easier for older adults to forget (deactivate) a PM intention that was performed repeatedly (4-target condition) than one that was never performed (0-target condition). However, older adults were more likely to make commission errors than young adults in the 4- but not the 0-target condition. Experiments 2 and 3 examined whether distinct strategies reduce commission errors. Whereas a preparatory instructional strategy produced inconsistent effects, forgetting practice was highly effective in producing floor levels of commission errors for older and young adults in the 4-target condition. Findings are interpreted within the dual-mechanisms account of PM commission errors, which highlights the interplay of spontaneous retrieval and cognitive control in the forgetting of previously relevant intentions. Practically, the findings provide first evidence of a translational strategy that older adults may use to minimize commission errors. (PsycINFO Database Record

[1]  Fergus I. M. Craik,et al.  A functional account of age differences in memory , 2016 .

[2]  U. Mayr,et al.  When and why do old adults outsource control to the environment? , 2015, Psychology and Aging.

[3]  Cynthia P. May,et al.  The best of both worlds: emotional cues improve prospective memory execution and reduce repetition errors , 2015, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[4]  Jan Rummel,et al.  Commission errors of active intentions: the roles of aging, cognitive load, and practice , 2015, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[5]  J. de Houwer,et al.  Learning through instructions vs. learning through practice: flanker congruency effects from instructed and applied S-R mappings , 2014, Psychological research.

[6]  F. Plessow,et al.  The role of temporal delay and repeated prospective memory cue exposure on the deactivation of completed intentions , 2014, Psychological research.

[7]  K. Bäuml,et al.  Don’t Do It Again! Directed Forgetting of Habits , 2014, Psychological science.

[8]  Julie M Bugg Evidence for the sparing of reactive cognitive control with age. , 2014, Psychology and aging.

[9]  J. Bugg,et al.  Conflict-triggered top-down control: default mode, last resort, or no such thing? , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  M. Scullin,et al.  Controlling Intentions , 2013, Psychological science.

[11]  G. Einstein,et al.  Prospective memory and aging: evidence for preserved spontaneous retrieval with exact but not related cues. , 2013, Psychology and aging.

[12]  Matthias Kliegel,et al.  Adult age differences, response management, and cue focality in event-based prospective memory: a meta-analysis on the role of task order specificity. , 2013, Psychology and aging.

[13]  M. Scullin,et al.  Failing to forget: prospective memory commission errors can result from spontaneous retrieval and impaired executive control. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  J. Houwer,et al.  Instruction-based response activation depends on task preparation , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  C. Dodson,et al.  Negative prospective memory: Remembering not to perform an action , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[16]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Strengthening encoding via implementation intention formation increases prospective memory commission errors , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[17]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Whoops, I did it again: commission errors in prospective memory. , 2012, Psychology and aging.

[18]  T. Goschke,et al.  The failure of deactivating intentions: aftereffects of completed intentions in the repeated prospective memory cue paradigm. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  P. Verhaeghen Aging and Executive Control: Reports of a Demise Greatly Exaggerated , 2011, Current directions in psychological science.

[20]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Prospective memory and aging: preserved spontaneous retrieval, but impaired deactivation, in older adults , 2011, Memory & cognition.

[21]  D. Stuss,et al.  Age-related differences in transfer costs: evidence from go/nogo tasks. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[22]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Repetition Errors in Habitual Prospective Memory: Elimination of Age Differences via Complex Actions or Appropriate Resource Allocation , 2009, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[23]  Todd S. Braver,et al.  Working Memory, Executive Control, and Aging , 2007 .

[24]  Christopher B Mayhorn,et al.  Comparing Older and Younger Adults in an Event-Based Prospective Memory Paradigm Containing an Output Monitoring Component , 2007, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[25]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Prospective Memory: An Overview and Synthesis of an Emerging Field , 2007 .

[26]  Robert Gross,et al.  The influence of patient adherence on anticoagulation control with warfarin: results from the International Normalized Ratio Adherence and Genetics (IN-RANGE) Study. , 2007, Archives of internal medicine.

[27]  L. Kvavilashvili,et al.  Is time-based prospective remembering mediated by self-initiated rehearsals? Role of incidental cues, ongoing activity, age, and motivation. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[28]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  The representation of instructions in working memory leads to autonomous response activation: Evidence from the first trials in the flanker paradigm , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[29]  Deanna M Barch,et al.  Effects of environmental support and strategy training on older adults' use of context. , 2006, Psychology and aging.

[30]  F. Craik,et al.  Cognition through the lifespan: mechanisms of change , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  R. Dixon,et al.  The Intention Interference Effect and Aging: Similar Magnitude of Effects for Young and Old Adults , 2005 .

[32]  Ajay B. Satpute,et al.  Context processing and context maintenance in healthy aging and early stage dementia of the Alzheimer's type. , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[33]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  J. Henry,et al.  A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[35]  K. Rothermund Automatic vigilance for task-related information: Perseverance after failure and inhibition after success , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[36]  F. Craik,et al.  Age-Related Differences in Cued Recall: Effects of Support at Encoding and Retrieval , 2002 .

[37]  Colleen M. Kelley,et al.  A contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[38]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[39]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Habitual Prospective Memory and Aging: Remembering Intentions and Forgetting Actions , 1998 .

[40]  Martin L. Bink,et al.  Activation of completed, uncompleted, and partially completed intentions , 1998 .

[41]  R. West,et al.  An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[42]  R. Roberts,et al.  Prefrontal cognitive processes: Working memory and inhibition in the antisaccade task. , 1994 .

[43]  T. Goschke,et al.  Representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. , 1993 .

[44]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Automatic versus intentional uses of memory: aging, attention, and control. , 1993, Psychology and aging.

[45]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Normal aging and prospective memory. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[46]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Inhibitory deficit theory: Recent developments in a "new view" , 2007 .

[47]  B. Hommel The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation , 2000 .

[48]  Faith M. Gunning-Dixon,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive aging: evidence from structural magnetic resonance imaging. , 1998, Neuropsychology.

[49]  L. Hasher,et al.  Directed ignoring: Inhibitory regulation of working memory. , 1994 .

[50]  D M Wegner,et al.  Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[51]  B. Zeigarnik On finished and unfinished tasks. , 1938 .