Social Models from Non-Human Systems

[1]  G. Box Science and Statistics , 1976 .

[2]  Daisuke Nakanishi,et al.  Cost–benefit analysis of social/cultural learning in a nonstationary uncertain environment: An evolutionary simulation and an experiment with human subjects , 2002 .

[3]  A. Mesoudi An experimental simulation of the “copy-successful-individuals” cultural learning strategy: adaptive landscapes, producer–scrounger dynamics, and informational access costs , 2008 .

[4]  Colin A. Chapman,et al.  Producers, Scroungers, and Group Foraging , 1991, The American Naturalist.

[5]  S. Pratt,et al.  Ants learn to rely on more informative attributes during decision-making , 2013, Biology Letters.

[6]  Jonathan N Pruitt,et al.  Frequency-Dependent Success of Cheaters During Foraging Bouts Might Limit Their Spread within Colonies of a Socially Polymorphic Spider , 2009, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[7]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  Food exploitation: searching for the optimal joining policy. , 1999, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[8]  Jean-Louis Deneubourg,et al.  Information Processing in Social Insects , 1999, Birkhäuser Basel.

[9]  W. Bialek,et al.  Are Biological Systems Poised at Criticality? , 2010, 1012.2242.

[10]  Charles J. Goodnight,et al.  Site-specific group selection drives locally adapted group compositions , 2014, Nature.

[11]  S. Pratt,et al.  Groups have a larger cognitive capacity than individuals , 2012, Current Biology.

[12]  S. Foitzik,et al.  Fitness costs of worker specialization for ant societies , 2016, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  Anna Dornhaus,et al.  Multimodal signals enhance decision making in foraging bumble-bees , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[14]  G. Box Robustness in the Strategy of Scientific Model Building. , 1979 .

[15]  Nigel R. Franks,et al.  The Use of Edges in Visual Navigation by the Ant Leptothorax albipennis , 2001 .

[16]  P. Visscher,et al.  House-hunting by honey bee swarms: collective decisions and individual behaviors , 1999, Insectes Sociaux.

[17]  Nigel R. Franks,et al.  Teaching in tandem-running ants , 2006, Nature.

[18]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  A test of the producer-scrounger foraging game in captive flocks of spice finches, Loncbura punctulata , 1994, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[19]  A. Banerjee,et al.  A Simple Model of Herd Behavior , 1992 .

[20]  Luc-Alain Giraldeau,et al.  Head position as an indicator of producer and scrounger tactics in a ground-feeding bird , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[21]  J. Deneubourg,et al.  Collective decision making through food recruitment , 1990, Insectes Sociaux.

[22]  A. Dussutour,et al.  Noise improves collective decision-making by ants in dynamic environments , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  I. Welch Sequential Sales, Learning, and Cascades , 1992 .

[24]  Iain D. Couzin,et al.  The Dynamics of Coordinated Group Hunting and Collective Information Transfer among Schooling Prey , 2012, Current Biology.

[25]  M. Brass,et al.  Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  Eamonn B. Mallon,et al.  Information flow, opinion polling and collective intelligence in house-hunting social insects. , 2002, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[27]  Michael Möglich,et al.  Tandem calling pheromone in the genusLeptothorax (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): Behavioral analysis of specificity , 2004, Journal of Chemical Ecology.

[28]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  Risky decisions: a test of risk sensitivity in socially foraging flocks of Lonchura punctulata , 2005 .

[29]  D. Sumpter,et al.  From nonlinearity to optimality: pheromone trail foraging by ants , 2003, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  Christopher G. Langton,et al.  Computation at the edge of chaos: Phase transitions and emergent computation , 1990 .

[31]  Ian Kuijt,et al.  People and Space in Early Agricultural Villages: Exploring Daily Lives, Community Size, and Architecture in the Late Pre-Pottery Neolithic , 2000 .

[32]  Laurent Lehmann,et al.  Coevolution of adaptive technology, maladaptive culture and population size in a producer–scrounger game , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  I. Coolen,et al.  Increasing foraging group size increases scrounger use and reduces searching efficiency in nutmeg mannikins (Lonchura punctulata) , 2002, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[34]  Lamjed Ben Said,et al.  Agent-Based Modeling and Simulation of the Emotional and Behavioral Dynamics of Human Civilians during Emergency Situations , 2014, MATES.

[35]  Richard P. Bagozzi,et al.  Specification, evaluation, and interpretation of structural equation models , 2012 .

[36]  C. Clark,et al.  Foraging and Flocking Strategies: Information in an Uncertain Environment , 1984, The American Naturalist.

[37]  Fred D. Davis,et al.  User Acceptance of Computer Technology: A Comparison of Two Theoretical Models , 1989 .

[38]  M. Moynihan,et al.  Hostile, Sexual, and Other Social Behaviour Patterns of the Spice Finch (Lonchura Punctulata) in Captivity , 1955 .

[39]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Wild Carib grackles play a producer-scrounger game , 2007 .

[40]  P. Visscher Group decision making in nest-site selection among social insects. , 2007, Annual review of entomology.

[41]  C. Harley Learning the evolutionarily stable strategy. , 1981, Journal of theoretical biology.

[42]  P. Groot Information transfer in a socially roosting weaver bird (Quelea quelea; Ploceinae): An experimental study , 1980, Animal Behaviour.

[43]  D. Biro,et al.  Cumulative culture can emerge from collective intelligence in animal groups , 2017, Nature Communications.

[44]  J. Deneubourg,et al.  Emerging patterns and food recruitment in ants: an analytical study , 1999, Journal of theoretical biology.

[45]  R. Sibly,et al.  Producers and scroungers: A general model and its application to captive flocks of house sparrows , 1981, Animal Behaviour.

[46]  Joshua M. Plotnik,et al.  Elephants know when they need a helping trunk in a cooperative task , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[47]  Frank C. Keil,et al.  The Shadows and Shallows of Explanation , 1998, Minds and Machines.

[48]  Fred D. Davis Perceived Usefulness, Perceived Ease of Use, and User Acceptance of Information Technology , 1989, MIS Q..

[49]  J. Pruitt,et al.  Sex matters: sexually dimorphic fitness consequences of a behavioural syndrome , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[50]  U. Maschwitz,et al.  Individual specific trails in the antLeptothorax affinis (Formicidae: Myrmicinae) , 1986, Experientia.

[51]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  Persistent individual differences in tactic use in a producer–scrounger game are group dependent , 2011, Animal Behaviour.

[52]  Dominic D. P. Johnson,et al.  The Origins and Evolution of Leadership , 2009, Current Biology.

[53]  M. Feigenbaum Quantitative universality for a class of nonlinear transformations , 1978 .

[54]  S. Bikhchandani,et al.  You have printed the following article : A Theory of Fads , Fashion , Custom , and Cultural Change as Informational Cascades , 2007 .

[55]  F. Bass A new product growth model for consumer durables , 1976 .

[56]  Takao Sasaki,et al.  Linear recruitment leads to allocation and flexibility in collective foraging by ants , 2013, Animal Behaviour.

[57]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  Experimental evidence that group foragers can converge on predicted producer–scrounger equilibria , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[58]  H. Reeve,et al.  The emergence of a superorganism through intergroup competition , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[59]  Theodore P. Pavlic,et al.  Superorganismic Behavior via Human Computation , 2013, Handbook of Human Computation.

[60]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Human-like social skills in dogs? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[61]  Rui Fan,et al.  An agent-based model for emotion contagion and competition in online social media , 2017, ArXiv.

[62]  J. Pruitt,et al.  Behavioural syndromes and their fitness consequences in a socially polymorphic spider, Anelosimus studiosus , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[63]  Nigel R. Franks,et al.  Blinkered teaching: tandem running by visually impaired ants , 2011, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[64]  J. Deneubourg,et al.  Modulation of trail laying in the antLasius niger (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) and its role in the collective selection of a food source , 1993, Journal of Insect Behavior.

[65]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Chimpanzee helping in collaborative and noncollaborative contexts , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[66]  M. Beekman,et al.  Keeping track of changes: the performance of ant colonies in dynamic environments , 2013, Animal Behaviour.

[67]  E Altshuler,et al.  Foraging at the edge of chaos: internal clock versus external forcing. , 2013, Physical review letters.

[68]  Behavioural traits of colony founders affect the life history of their colonies. , 2012, Ecology letters.

[69]  M. M�glich,et al.  Tandem Calling: A New Kind of Signal in Ant Communication , 1974, Science.