Optimal and Suboptimal Use of Postsaccadic Vision in Sequences of Saccades

Saccades are imprecise, due to sensory and motor noise. To avoid an accumulation of errors during sequences of saccades, a prediction derived from the efference copy can be combined with the reafferent visual feedback to adjust the following eye movement. By varying the information quantity of the visual feedback, we investigated how the reliability of the visual information affects the postsaccadic update in humans. Two elements of the visual scene were manipulated, the saccade target or the background, presented either together or in isolation. We determined the weight of the postsaccadic visual information by measuring the effect of intrasaccadic visual shifts on the following saccade. We confirmed that the weight of visual information evolves with information quantity as predicted for a statistically optimal system. In particular, we found that the visual background alone can guide the postsaccadic update, and that information from target and background are optimally combined. Moreover, these visual weights are adjusted dynamically and on a trial-to-trial basis to the level of visual noise determined by target eccentricity and reaction time. In contrast, we uncovered a dissociation between the visual signals used to update the next planned saccade (main saccade) and those used to generate an involuntary corrective saccade. The latter was exclusively based on visual information about the target, and discarded all information about the background: a suboptimal use of visual evidence.

[1]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. , 1969, Vision research.

[2]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  The Sources of Variability in Saccadic Eye Movements , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[4]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Neurons in the monkey superior colliculus predict the visual result of impending saccadic eye movements. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  David C Knill,et al.  Visual Feedback Control of Hand Movements , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  D. Henson Corrective saccades: Effects of altering visual feedback , 1978, Vision Research.

[7]  D. J. Brown,et al.  Peripheral visual acuity. , 1966, Archives of ophthalmology.

[8]  James M. Hillis,et al.  Combining Sensory Information: Mandatory Fusion Within, but Not Between, Senses , 2002, Science.

[9]  John M. Findlay,et al.  Foveal stimulation and saccadic latencies , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  Yoshiko Kojima,et al.  Complex spike activity in the oculomotor vermis of the cerebellum: a vectorial error signal for saccade motor learning? , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Robert H. Wurtz,et al.  Influence of the thalamus on spatial visual processing in frontal cortex , 2006, Nature.

[12]  Pierre Baraduc,et al.  Saccadic adaptation without retinal postsaccadic error , 2007, Neuroreport.

[13]  L E Mays,et al.  Saccades are spatially, not retinocentrically, coded. , 1980, Science.

[14]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Characteristics of saccadic gain adaptation in rhesus macaques. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Mingsha Zhang,et al.  The proprioceptive representation of eye position in monkey primary somatosensory cortex , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[17]  D. Pélisson,et al.  On-line modification of saccadic eye movements by retinal signals , 2003, Neuroreport.

[18]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.

[19]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. III. Maintenance of a spatially accurate saccade signal. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  C. Colby,et al.  Spatial updating in area LIP is independent of saccade direction. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[22]  D. Robinson,et al.  Saccadic undershoot is not inevitable: Saccades can be accurate , 1986, Vision Research.

[23]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[24]  A. Kuo An optimal state estimation model of sensory integration in human postural balance , 2005, Journal of neural engineering.

[25]  F N LOW Peripheral visual acuity of 55 subjects under conditions of flash presentation. , 1947, The American journal of physiology.

[26]  G. Westheimer Eye movement responses to a horizontally moving visual stimulus. , 1954, A.M.A. archives of ophthalmology.

[27]  L L SLOAN,et al.  Peripheral visual acuity with special reference to scotopic illumination. , 1947, American journal of ophthalmology.

[28]  W. Shebilske Extraretinal information in corrective saccades and inflow vs outflow theories of visual direction constancy , 1976, Vision Research.

[29]  R. Wurtz,et al.  A Pathway in Primate Brain for Internal Monitoring of Movements , 2002, Science.

[30]  Jean-René Duhamel,et al.  Optimal Sensorimotor Control in Eye Movement Sequences , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  S. Klein,et al.  Position sense of the peripheral retina. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[32]  Emmanuel Guigon,et al.  Active control of bias for the control of posture and movement. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  M. Jeasnerod CORRECTIVE SACCADES : DEPENDENCE ON RETINAL REAFFERENT SIGNALS , 2008 .

[34]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Why Does the Brain Predict Sensory Consequences of Oculomotor Commands? Optimal Integration of the Predicted and the Actual Sensory Feedback , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Aditya Murthy,et al.  Frontal eye field contributions to rapid corrective saccades. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  R. E. Kalman,et al.  New Results in Linear Filtering and Prediction Theory , 1961 .

[37]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Spatial processing in the monkey frontal eye field. I. Predictive visual responses. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Saccades to remembered target locations: an analysis of systematic and variable errors , 1994, Vision Research.

[39]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[40]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.