Childhood IQ, smoking, and cognitive change from age 11 to 64 years.

We investigated whether smoking is a risk factor for relative cognitive decline from age 11 to 64 years. The potentially confounding effects of childhood IQ, occupational status, level of education, presence of heart disease, hypertension, and lung function were examined. Subjects were nondemented and living independently. They were all born in 1936, had been participants in the same Scottish national IQ survey in 1947, and were reexamined at age about 64 years in 2000-2002. Current smokers and nonsmokers had significantly different mental test scores at age 64. This difference remained after adjustment for childhood IQ. Multiple linear regression identified childhood IQ, level of education, occupational code, lung function, and smoking history as significant independent predictors of mental function at age 64. In this sample, smoking makes a small (<1% variance) independent negative contribution to cognitive aging.

[1]  R Doll,et al.  Smoking and dementia in male British doctors: prospective study , 2000, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[2]  A. Hofman,et al.  Smoking and risk of dementia and Alzheimer's disease in a population-based cohort study: the Rotterdam Study , 1998, The Lancet.

[3]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[4]  J. Gallacher,et al.  Smoking, drinking, and other life style factors and cognitive function in men in the Caerphilly cohort. , 1999, Journal of epidemiology and community health.

[5]  T. Salthouse The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. , 1996, Psychological review.

[6]  K J Anstey,et al.  Interrelationships among biological markers of aging, health, activity, acculturation, and cognitive performance in late adulthood. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[7]  I. Deary,et al.  Smoking and cognitive change from age 11 to age 80 , 2003, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[8]  T. Tombaugh,et al.  The Mini‐Mental State Examination: A Comprehensive Review , 1992, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[9]  P. Rubé,et al.  L’examen Clinique en Psychologie , 1959 .

[10]  M. Siegel,et al.  Mass Media Antismoking Campaigns: A Powerful Tool for Health Promotion , 1998, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[11]  M. Lezak Neuropsychological assessment, 3rd ed. , 1995 .

[12]  J. Jolles,et al.  Cigarette smoking and alcohol consumption in relation to cognitive performance in middle age. , 2002, American journal of epidemiology.

[13]  S A Glantz,et al.  Evaluation of antismoking advertising campaigns. , 1998, JAMA.

[14]  J. Rusted,et al.  Effortful processing is a requirement for nicotine-induced improvements in memory , 1998, Psychopharmacology.

[15]  Ian J Deary,et al.  The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: following up the Scottish mental surveys of 1932 and 1947. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  H. S. Koelega Stimulant drugs and vigilance performance: a review , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[17]  M. Jarvis,et al.  Cigarette smoking and cognitive decline in midlife: evidence from a prospective birth cohort study. , 2003, American journal of public health.

[18]  Residual effects of cigarette smoking on cognitive performance in normal aging. , 1989 .

[19]  A. Mann,et al.  Smoking, drinking, and incident cognitive impairment: a cohort community based study included in the Gospel Oak project , 2000, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[20]  C. Pomerleau,et al.  Euphoriant effects of nicotine , 1994 .

[21]  V M Hawthorne,et al.  Impaired lung function and mortality risk in men and women: findings from the Renfrew and Paisley prospective population study , 1996, BMJ.

[22]  L. Flicker,et al.  Smoking as a risk factor for Alzheimer's disease: contrasting evidence from a systematic review of case-control and cohort studies. , 2002, Addiction.