Differentiating between integration and non-integration strategies in perceptual decision making

Many tasks used to study decision-making encourage subjects to integrate evidence over time. Such tasks are useful to understand how the brain operates on multiple samples of information over prolonged timescales, but only if subjects actually integrate evidence to form their decisions. We explored the behavioral observations that corroborate evidence-integration in a number of task-designs. Several commonly accepted signs of integration were also predicted by non-integration strategies. Furthermore, an integration model could fit data generated by non-integration models. We identified the features of non-integration models that allowed them to mimic integration and used these insights to design a motion discrimination task that disentangled the models. In human subjects performing the task, we falsified a non-integration strategy in each and confirmed prolonged integration in all but one subject. The findings illustrate the difficulty of identifying a decision-maker’s strategy and support solutions to achieve this goal.

[1]  J. Gold,et al.  Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands , 2000, Nature.

[2]  Ben Deverett,et al.  Cerebellar involvement in an evidence-accumulation decision-making task , 2018, eLife.

[3]  R. Ratcliff Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[4]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Elapsed Decision Time Affects the Weighting of Prior Probability in a Perceptual Decision Task , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Il Memming Park,et al.  Functional dissection of signal and noise in MT and LIP during decision-making , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Practice increases the efficiency of evidence accumulation in perceptual choice. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Rhythmic Fluctuations in Evidence Accumulation during Decision Making in the Human Brain , 2012, Neuron.

[8]  Jochen Ditterich,et al.  Stochastic models of decisions about motion direction: Behavior and physiology , 2006, Neural Networks.

[9]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Discriminating evidence accumulation from urgency signals in speeded decision making. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Temporal Integration of Olfactory Perceptual Evidence in Human Orbitofrontal Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[11]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[13]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  Comparison of Decision-Related Signals in Sensory and Motor Preparatory Responses of Neurons in Area LIP , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Decision making by urgency gating: theory and experimental support. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Paul Cisek,et al.  The urgency-gating model can explain the effects of early evidence , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[16]  Gouki Okazawa,et al.  Psychophysical reverse correlation reflects both sensory and decision-making processes , 2018, Nature Communications.

[17]  R. Whelan Effective Analysis of Reaction Time Data , 2008 .

[18]  Bingni W. Brunton,et al.  Rats and Humans Can Optimally Accumulate Evidence for Decision-Making , 2013, Science.

[19]  J. Townsend,et al.  Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. , 1993, Psychological review.

[20]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Bounded Integration in Parietal Cortex Underlies Decisions Even When Viewing Duration Is Dictated by the Environment , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  J. S. Chang,et al.  A practical difference scheme for Fokker-Planck equations☆ , 1970 .

[22]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Representation of Confidence Associated with a Decision by Neurons in the Parietal Cortex , 2009, Science.

[23]  Ari S. Morcos,et al.  History-dependent variability in population dynamics during evidence accumulation in cortex , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Piercing of Consciousness as a Threshold-Crossing Operation , 2017, Current Biology.

[25]  P. Cisek,et al.  Decisions in Changing Conditions: The Urgency-Gating Model , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Michael L. Waskom,et al.  Decision Making through Integration of Sensory Evidence at Prolonged Timescales , 2018, Current Biology.

[27]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[28]  E. Rolls,et al.  Brain mechanisms for perceptual and reward-related decision-making , 2013, Progress in Neurobiology.

[29]  Luigi Acerbi,et al.  Variational Bayesian Monte Carlo , 2018, NeurIPS.

[30]  Mehrdad Jazayeri,et al.  Representation of Accumulating Evidence for a Decision in Two Parietal Areas , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  M N Shadlen,et al.  Motion perception: seeing and deciding. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  Onyekachi Odoemene,et al.  Visual Evidence Accumulation Guides Decision-Making in Unrestrained Mice , 2017, Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Bingni W. Brunton,et al.  Distinct relationships of parietal and prefrontal cortices to evidence accumulation , 2014, Nature.

[34]  L. Festinger,et al.  A quantitative theory of decision , 1943 .

[35]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural Activity in Macaque Parietal Cortex Reflects Temporal Integration of Visual Motion Signals during Perceptual Decision Making , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Guy E. Hawkins,et al.  When humans behave like monkeys: Feedback delays and extensive practice increase the efficiency of speeded decisions , 2019, Cognition.

[37]  Max C. Keuken,et al.  Early evidence affects later decisions: Why evidence accumulation is required to explain response time data , 2014, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[39]  Eric Shea-Brown,et al.  Neural integrators for decision making: a favorable tradeoff between robustness and sensitivity , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[41]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Decision Making as a Window on Cognition , 2013, Neuron.

[42]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The computations that support simple decision-making: A comparison between the diffusion and urgency-gating models , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[43]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Evidence against perfect integration of sensory information during perceptual decision making. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  A. Watson Probability summation over time , 1979, Vision Research.

[45]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[46]  Luigi Acerbi,et al.  Practical Bayesian Optimization for Model Fitting with Bayesian Adaptive Direct Search , 2017, NIPS.

[47]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[48]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Computational psychiatry , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[49]  Christopher R Fetsch,et al.  The influence of evidence volatility on choice, reaction time and confidence in a perceptual decision , 2016, eLife.

[50]  T. Branco,et al.  A synaptic threshold mechanism for computing escape decisions , 2018, Nature.

[51]  Ben Deverett,et al.  An Accumulation-of-Evidence Task Using Visual Pulses for Mice Navigating in Virtual Reality , 2017, bioRxiv.

[52]  Jonathan W. Pillow,et al.  Dissociated functional significance of decision-related activity in the primate dorsal stream , 2016, Nature.

[53]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Importance of Falsification in Computational Cognitive Modeling , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.