A Neurocognitive Framework for Human Creative Thought

We are an intensely creative species. Creativity is the fountainhead of our civilizations and a defining characteristic of what makes us human. But for all its prominence at the apex of human mental faculties, we know next to nothing about how brains generate creative ideas. With all previous attempts to tighten the screws on this vexed problem unsuccessful – right brains, divergent thinking, defocused attention, default mode network, alpha enhancement, prefrontal activation, etc. (Dietrich and Kanso, 2010) – the neuroscientific study of creativity finds itself in a theoretical arid zone that has perhaps no equal in psychology. We propose here a general framework for a fresh attack on the problem and set it out under 10 foundational concepts. Most of the ideas we favor are part and parcel of the standard conceptual toolbox of cognitive neuroscience but their combination and significance to creativity are original. By outlining, even in such broad strokes, the theoretical landscape of cognitive neuroscience as it relates to creative insights, we hope to bring into clear focus the key enabling factors that are likely to have a hand in computing ideational combinations in the brain.

[1]  D. Schacter,et al.  Creative Cognition and Brain Network Dynamics , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  H. Haider,et al.  Human creativity, evolutionary algorithms, and predictive representations: The mechanics of thought trials , 2014, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[3]  Mark A. Runco,et al.  The Neuroscience of Divergent Thinking , 2014 .

[4]  Arne Dietrich,et al.  How Creativity Happens in the Brain , 2014 .

[5]  A. Clark Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[6]  Michael Öllinger,et al.  Cognitive mechanisms of insight: the role of heuristics and representational change in solving the eight-coin problem. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  Robert Kurzban,et al.  Cognitive systems for revenge and forgiveness. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[8]  R. Weisberg On the “Demystification” of Insight: A Critique of Neuroimaging Studies of Insight , 2013 .

[9]  Randy L. Buckner,et al.  The serendipitous discovery of the brain's default network , 2012, NeuroImage.

[10]  Dean Keith Simonton,et al.  Taking the U.S. Patent Office Criteria Seriously: A Quantitative Three-Criterion Creativity Definition and Its Implications , 2012 .

[11]  H. Haider,et al.  An old problem: How can we distinguish between conscious and unconscious knowledge acquired in an implicit learning task? , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[12]  J. Changeux,et al.  Experimental and Theoretical Approaches to Conscious Processing , 2011, Neuron.

[13]  K. Sawyer The Cognitive Neuroscience of Creativity: A Critical Review , 2011 .

[14]  C. Büchel,et al.  The emergence of explicit memory during learning. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  A. Dietrich,et al.  A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and insight. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[16]  Maria E. Kronfeldner,et al.  Darwinian ‘blind’ hypothesis formation revisited , 2010, Synthese.

[17]  S. Bressler,et al.  Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  Hilde Haider,et al.  Conflicts between expected and actually performed behavior lead to verbal report of incidentally acquired sequential knowledge , 2009, Psychological research.

[19]  Samuel T. Moulton,et al.  Imagining predictions: mental imagery as mental emulation , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  Sisir Roy,et al.  The ‘prediction imperative’ as the basis for self-awareness , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  Keith L. Downing,et al.  Predictive models in the brain , 2009, Connect. Sci..

[22]  H. Haider,et al.  How task representations guide attention: further evidence for the shielding function of task sets. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  M. Bar The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  A. Dietrich,et al.  Who's afraid of a cognitive neuroscience of creativity? , 2007, Methods.

[25]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[26]  Keith Jones,et al.  Not by genes alone , 2006 .

[27]  Arne Dietrich,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the experience of flow , 2004, Consciousness and Cognition.

[28]  A. Dietrich,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of creativity , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[29]  Rick Grush,et al.  The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[30]  K. Doya,et al.  A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[31]  A. Vinter,et al.  The self-organizing consciousness. , 2002, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[32]  Annie Vinter,et al.  The self-organizing consciousness as an alternative model of the mind , 2002 .

[33]  Josef Perner,et al.  A theory of the implicit nature of implicit learning , 2002 .

[34]  G. Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[35]  W. Schultz Multiple reward signals in the brain , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  D. Solter Mammalian cloning: advances and limitations , 2000, Nature Reviews Genetics.

[37]  S. Ohlsson,et al.  Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving , 1999 .

[38]  Ingar Brinck,et al.  The developmental progression from implicit to explicit knowledge: A computational approach , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[39]  A. Miyake,et al.  Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control , 1999 .

[40]  N. Cowan An embedded-processes model of working memory , 1999 .

[41]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[42]  G. Ferro-Luzzi On Evolutionary Epistemology , 1982, Current Anthropology.

[43]  D. Campbell Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. , 1960, Psychological review.

[44]  Nelson Cowan,et al.  Working Memory Capacity , 2005 .

[45]  A. Allport,et al.  Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.

[46]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Introduction to Consciousness , 1995 .

[47]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[48]  A. Reber Implicit learning and tacit knowledge , 1993 .

[49]  B. Baars A cognitive theory of consciousness , 1988 .

[50]  Odmar Neumann,et al.  Automatic Processing: A Review of Recent Findings and a Plea for an Old Theory , 1984 .

[51]  J. Pollard Evolutionary theory : paths into the future , 1984 .

[52]  D. Campbell Unjustified Variation and Selective Retention in Scientific Discovery , 1974 .

[53]  D. Simonton Creativity as Blind Variation and Selective Retention : Is the Creative Process Darwinian ? , 2022 .

[54]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .