Tool use and affordance: Manipulation-based versus reasoning-based approaches.

Tool use is a defining feature of human species. Therefore, a fundamental issue is to understand the cognitive bases of human tool use. Given that people cannot use tools without manipulating them, proponents of the manipulation-based approach have argued that tool use might be supported by the simulation of past sensorimotor experiences, also sometimes called affordances. However, in the meanwhile, evidence has been accumulated demonstrating the critical role of mechanical knowledge in tool use (i.e., the reasoning-based approach). The major goal of the present article is to examine the validity of the assumptions derived from the manipulation-based versus the reasoning-based approach. To do so, we identified 3 key issues on which the 2 approaches differ, namely, (a) the reference frame issue, (b) the intention issue, and (c) the action domain issue. These different issues will be addressed in light of studies in experimental psychology and neuropsychology that have provided valuable contributions to the topic (i.e., tool-use interaction, orientation effect, object-size effect, utilization behavior and anarchic hand, tool use and perception, apraxia of tool use, transport vs. use actions). To anticipate our conclusions, the reasoning-based approach seems to be promising for understanding the current literature, even if it is not fully satisfactory because of a certain number of findings easier to interpret with regard to the manipulation-based approach. A new avenue for future research might be to develop a framework accommodating both approaches, thereby shedding a new light on the cognitive bases of human tool use and affordances. (PsycINFO Database Record

[1]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[2]  H. Liepmann Drei Aufsätze aus dem Apraxiegebiet , 1908 .

[3]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[4]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Reactions toward the source of stimulation. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: with special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. , 1970, Psychological review.

[6]  J. J. Gibson The theory of affordances , 1977 .

[7]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[8]  Steven L. Johnson Effect of Training Device on Retention and Transfer of a Procedural Task , 1981 .

[9]  K. Heilman,et al.  Two forms of ideomotor apraxia , 1982, Neurology.

[10]  F. Lhermitte 'Utilization behaviour' and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  W H Warren,et al.  Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  E. Roy,et al.  Common Considerations In The Study of Limb, Verbal And Oral Apraxia , 1985 .

[13]  K. Heilman,et al.  Apraxia After a Superior Parietal Lesion , 1986, Cortex.

[14]  B. Pillon,et al.  Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part I: Imitation and utilization behavior: A neuropsychological study of 75 patients , 1986, Annals of neurology.

[15]  W. Warren,et al.  Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  C. Michaels S-R compatibility between response position and destination of apparent motion: Evidence of the detection of affordances. , 1988 .

[17]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The timing of mentally represented actions , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[18]  M. Turvey,et al.  Visually perceiving what is reachable. , 1989 .

[19]  K. Heilman,et al.  Ideational apraxia: A deficit in tool selection and use , 1989, Annals of neurology.

[20]  T. Shallice,et al.  The origins of utilization behaviour. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  T. G. Reeve,et al.  Research on Stimulus-Response Compatibility: Toward a Comprehensive Account , 1990 .

[22]  E. A. Alluisi,et al.  Things That Go Together: A Review of Stimulus-Response Compatibility and Related Effects , 1990 .

[23]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[24]  K. Heilman,et al.  A Cognitive Neuropsychological Model of Limb Praxis , 1991 .

[25]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition. A case of multimodal agnosia. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  H. Spinnler,et al.  Right-sided anarchic (alien) hand: A longitudinal study , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  The Development of Young Infants' Intuitions about Support , 1992 .

[28]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Time course of movement planning: selection of handgrips for object manipulation. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  M. Hallett,et al.  Procedural learning in Parkinson's disease and cerebellar degeneration , 1993, Annals of neurology.

[30]  C. Michaels Destination Compatibility, Affordances, and Coding Rules: A Reply to Proctor, Van Zandt, Lu, and Weeks , 1993 .

[31]  B. Hommel Inverting the Simon effect intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. , 1993 .

[32]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). , 1994, Journal of comparative psychology.

[33]  H. Spinnler,et al.  Spared and Impaired Cognitive Abilities After Bilateral Frontal Damage , 1994, Cortex.

[34]  R. Proctor,et al.  The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[35]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[36]  E. Renzi,et al.  Imitation and utilisation behaviour. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[37]  Pierre Mounoud,et al.  A recursive transformation of central cognitive mechanisms : the shift from partial to whole representations , 1996 .

[38]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[39]  T Shallice,et al.  The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[40]  B. Wood The oldest whodunnit in the world , 1997, Nature.

[41]  W. Prinz Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .

[42]  A. Glenberg,et al.  What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[43]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  The Role of Semantic Memory in Object Use , 1997 .

[44]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective Reaching to Grasp: Evidence for Distractor Interference Effects , 1997 .

[45]  T. Shallice,et al.  Modality-Specific Operations in Semantic Dementia , 1997, Cortex.

[46]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Recognition by action: dissociating visual and semantic routes to action in normal observers. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  G. Goldenberg,et al.  Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Visual affordances direct action: neuropsychological evidence from manual interference. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[49]  R. Ellis,et al.  On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  J R Hodges,et al.  "What" and "how": evidence for the dissociation of object knowledge and mechanical problem-solving skills in the human brain. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[51]  D. Proffitt,et al.  Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  K. Heilman,et al.  Apraxia and motor-skill acquisition in Alzheimersdisease are dissociable , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[54]  M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al.  The role of conceptual knowledge in object use evidence from semantic dementia. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[55]  T. Shallice,et al.  CONTENTION SCHEDULING AND THE CONTROL OF ROUTINE ACTIVITIES , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[56]  R. Ellis,et al.  Micro-affordance: the potentiation of components of action by seen objects. , 2000, British journal of psychology.

[57]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The Role of Semantic Knowledge and Working Memory in Everyday Tasks , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[58]  R. Cubelli,et al.  Cognition in Action: Testing a Model of Limb Apraxia , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[59]  D. Povinelli Folk physics for apes , 2000 .

[60]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  The Role of the Dynamic Body Schema in Praxis: Evidence from Primary Progressive Apraxia , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[61]  Michael R. Neiman,et al.  The assessment of limb apraxia: Relationship between performances on single- and multiple-object tasks by left hemisphere damaged aphasic subjects , 2000 .

[62]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[63]  R. Ellis,et al.  The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization , 2001 .

[64]  L. Buxbaum Ideomotor Apraxia: a Call to Action , 2001, Neurocase.

[65]  M. Jeannerod Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.

[66]  J. Riddoch,et al.  Visual affordances and object selection , 2001 .

[67]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Posture-based motion planning: applications to grasping. , 2001, Psychological review.

[68]  S. Anderson,et al.  Attentional processes link perception and action , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[69]  R. Ward,et al.  S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: Time course and specificity of response activation , 2002 .

[70]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and nonapraxic subjects , 2002, Brain and Language.

[71]  J. Decety,et al.  Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[72]  J. Hodges,et al.  When objects lose their meaning: What happens to their use? , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[73]  J. Hodges,et al.  Apraxia, mechanical problem solving and semantic knowledge , 2002, Journal of Neurology.

[74]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Modelling direct perceptual constraints on action selection: The Naming and Action Model (NAM) , 2002 .

[75]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Visual Search Is Modulated by Action Intentions , 2002, Psychological science.

[76]  Robert E. Shaw,et al.  The Agent-Environment Interface: Simon's Indirect or Gibson's Direct Coupling? , 2003 .

[77]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Cognitive representations of hand posture in ideomotor apraxia , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[78]  R. Passingham,et al.  Objects automatically potentiate action: an fMRI study of implicit processing , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[79]  David B. Miller,et al.  Nested reciprocities: the organism-environment system in perception-action and development. , 2003, Developmental psychobiology.

[80]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[81]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Grasping the meaning of words , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[82]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  The Role of Effort in Perceiving Distance , 2003, Psychological science.

[83]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey What's So Special about Human Tool Use? , 2003, Neuron.

[84]  A. Borghi Object concepts and action: extracting affordances from objects parts. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[85]  R. Ellis,et al.  Action priming by briefly presented objects. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[86]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Neural basis of pantomiming the use of visually presented objects , 2004, NeuroImage.

[87]  John R Anderson,et al.  An integrated theory of the mind. , 2004, Psychological review.

[88]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey The neural bases of complex tool use in humans , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[89]  A. Maravita,et al.  Tools for the body (schema) , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[90]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The motor theory of social cognition: a critique , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[91]  W. Epstein,et al.  Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[92]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[93]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Direct and indirect effects of action on object classification , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[94]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  A distributed left hemisphere network active during planning of everyday tool use skills. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[95]  A. Borghi Grounding Cognition: Object Concepts and Action , 2005 .

[96]  J. Hermsdörfer,et al.  It takes the whole brain to make a cup of coffee: the neuropsychology of naturalistic actions involving technical devices , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[97]  Ed Symes,et al.  Dissociating object-based and space-based affordances , 2005 .

[98]  D. Proffitt Embodied Perception and the Economy of Action , 2006, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[99]  Tao Wu,et al.  The role of the dorsal stream for gesture production , 2006, NeuroImage.

[100]  A. Sirigu,et al.  How we interact with objects: learning from brain lesions , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[101]  S. Tipper,et al.  Vision-for-action: The effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[102]  Julie C. Sedivy,et al.  Playing on the typewriter, typing on the piano: manipulation knowledge of objects , 2006, Cognition.

[103]  R. Ellis,et al.  The potentiation of two components of the reach-to-grasp action during object categorisation in visual memory. , 2006, Acta psychologica.

[104]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Neural substrates of knowledge of hand postures for object grasping and functional object use: Evidence from fMRI , 2006, Brain Research.

[105]  Markus Graf,et al.  The role of action representations in visual object recognition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[106]  G. Goldenberg,et al.  Relationship between Object-Related Gestures and the Fractionated Object Knowledge System , 2007, Behavioural neurology.

[107]  B. Hommel,et al.  Controlling attention through action: Observing actions primes action-related stimulus dimensions , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[108]  Derek C. Penn,et al.  Causal cognition in human and nonhuman animals: a comparative, critical review. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[109]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[110]  W. Prinz,et al.  Programming tool-use actions. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[111]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Dissociative effects of viewpoint and semantic priming on action and semantic decisions: Evidence for dual routes to action from vision , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[112]  James N. Lee,et al.  The influence of complex action knowledge on representations of novel graspable objects: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2007, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[113]  W. Prinz,et al.  Perceptual resonance: action-induced modulation of perception , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[114]  G. Gigli,et al.  Degraded Semantic Knowledge And Accurate Object Use , 2007, Cortex.

[115]  Amy M. Masnick,et al.  The Development of Causal Reasoning , 2007 .

[116]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  Toward a new theory of motor synergies. , 2007, Motor control.

[117]  Adrienne D. Moll,et al.  Abnormal reliance on object structure in apraxics' learning of novel object-related actions , 2007, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[118]  T. Chaminade,et al.  The evolutionary neuroscience of tool making , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[119]  Ed Symes,et al.  Visual object affordances: object orientation. , 2007, Acta psychologica.

[120]  G. Goldenberg,et al.  The Impact of Dysexecutive Syndrome on Use of Tools and Technical Devices , 2007, Cortex.

[121]  W. Prinz,et al.  Activation of action rules in action observation. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[122]  D. Bub,et al.  Kicking Calculators: Contribution of Embodied Representations to Sentence Comprehension. , 2008 .

[123]  F. Osiurak,et al.  Different constraints on grip selection in brain-damaged patients: Object use versus object transport , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[124]  George S. Cree,et al.  Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words , 2008, Cognition.

[125]  F. Osiurak,et al.  Object utilization and object usage: A single-case study , 2008, Neurocase.

[126]  D. Proffitt,et al.  Action-specific influences on distance perception: a role for motor simulation. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[127]  L. Riggio,et al.  The role of attention in the occurrence of the affordance effect. , 2008, Acta Psychologica.

[128]  L. Vainio,et al.  On the relations between action planning, object identification, and motor representations of observed actions and objects , 2008, Cognition.

[129]  A. Mack,et al.  Potentiation of action by undetected affordant objects , 2008 .

[130]  E. Biryukova,et al.  Organization of goal-directed action at a high level of motor skill: the case of stone knapping in India. , 2008, Motor control.

[131]  Guy Vingerhoets,et al.  Knowing about tools: Neural correlates of tool familiarity and experience , 2008, NeuroImage.

[132]  Derek C. Penn,et al.  Darwin's mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[133]  John K. Tsotsos,et al.  An Attentional Mechanism for Selecting Appropriate Actions Afforded by Graspable Objects , 2008, Psychological science.

[134]  L. Vainio,et al.  Grasp preparation improves change detection for congruent objects. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[135]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Action observation can prime visual object recognition , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[136]  Lucia Riggio,et al.  Sentence comprehension and simulation of object temporary, canonical and stable affordances , 2009, Brain Research.

[137]  Lawrence W Barsalou,et al.  Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[138]  M. Silveri,et al.  Semantic memory in object use , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[139]  G. Vingerhoets,et al.  Conceptual and physical object qualities contribute differently to motor affordances , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[140]  G. Goldenberg Apraxia and the parietal lobes , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[141]  G. Goldenberg,et al.  The neural basis of tool use. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[142]  J. Besnard,et al.  Contrôle exécutif et comportement d'utilisation d'objets : vers une dissociation , 2009 .

[143]  W. Prinz,et al.  Movements, actions and tool-use actions: an ideomotor approach to imitation , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[144]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  The effects of handedness and reachability on perceived distance. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[145]  G. Orban,et al.  The Representation of Tool Use in Humans and Monkeys: Common and Uniquely Human Features , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[146]  F. Osiurak,et al.  Unusual use of objects after unilateral brain damage. The technical reasoning model , 2009, Cortex.

[147]  Alice C. Roy,et al.  Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema , 2009, Current Biology.

[148]  B. Hommel Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding) , 2009, Psychological research.

[149]  Dietmar Heinke,et al.  MODELING VISUAL AFFORDANCES: THE SELECTIVE ATTENTION FOR ACTION MODEL (SAAM) , 2009 .

[150]  Gilles Dietrich,et al.  The role of expertise in tool use: skill differences in functional action adaptations to task constraints. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[151]  R. Nicoletti,et al.  Simon-Like and Functional Affordance Effects with Tools: The Effects of Object Perceptual Discrimination and Object Action State , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[152]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment in the two action systems , 2010, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[153]  Lewis A. Wheaton,et al.  The Neuroscience of Storing and Molding Tool Action Concepts: How “Plastic” is Grounded Cognition? , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[154]  Ed Symes,et al.  When Motor Attention Improves Selective Attention: The Dissociating Role of Saliency , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[155]  D. Bub,et al.  Grasping beer mugs: on the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[156]  Steven A. Jax,et al.  Response interference between functional and structural actions linked to the same familiar object , 2010, Cognition.

[157]  R. Proctor,et al.  The object-based Simon effect: grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[158]  R. Nicoletti,et al.  On the relationship between affordance and Simon effects: Are the effects really independent? , 2011 .

[159]  D. Parisi,et al.  TRoPICALS: a computational embodied neuroscience model of compatibility effects. , 2010, Psychological review.

[160]  Sally A. Linkenauger,et al.  A Functional Role for Motor Simulation in Identifying Tools , 2010, Psychological science.

[161]  F. Osiurak,et al.  Grasping the affordances, understanding the reasoning: toward a dialectical theory of human tool use. , 2010, Psychological review.

[162]  S. Tipper,et al.  Gesturing Meaning: Non-action Words Activate the Motor System , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[163]  R. Bongers,et al.  Joint angle variability and co-variation in a reaching with a rod task , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[164]  Julie C. Sedivy,et al.  Impaired access to manipulation features in Apraxia: Evidence from eyetracking and semantic judgment tasks , 2010, Brain and Language.

[165]  Jonathan R. Zadra,et al.  Direct Evidence for the Economy of Action: Glucose and the Perception of Geographical Slant , 2010, Perception.

[166]  D. Le Gall,et al.  Utilization behavior: Clinical and theoretical approaches , 2010, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[167]  Gaetano Tieri,et al.  Where does an object trigger an action? An investigation about affordances in space , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[168]  T. Shallice,et al.  Manipulability and object recognition: is manipulability a semantic feature? , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[169]  Craig S. Chapman,et al.  To use or to move: goal-set modulates priming when grasping real tools , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[170]  F. Osiurak,et al.  Re-examining the gesture engram hypothesis. New perspectives on apraxia of tool use , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[171]  J. C. Mizelle,et al.  Testing perceptual limits of functional units: Are there “automatic” tendencies to associate tools and objects? , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[172]  D. Gall,et al.  A contribution to the study of environmental dependency phenomena: The social hypothesis , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[173]  R. Proctor,et al.  Correspondence effects for objects with opposing left and right protrusions. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[174]  V. Gallese,et al.  Tool-use observation makes far objects ready-to-hand , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[175]  D. Bub,et al.  Priming of reach and grasp actions by handled objects. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[176]  Domenico Parisi,et al.  Objects, spatial compatibility, and affordances: A connectionist study , 2011, Cognitive Systems Research.

[177]  G. Vingerhoets,et al.  Neural correlates of pantomiming familiar and unfamiliar tools: Action semantics versus mechanical problem solving? , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[178]  K. Yarrow,et al.  Viewing objects and planning actions: On the potentiation of grasping behaviours by visual objects , 2011, Brain and Cognition.

[179]  E. Brenner,et al.  Using a Stick Does Not Necessarily Alter Judged Distances or Reachability , 2011, PloS one.

[180]  S. Tipper,et al.  Object affordance and spatial-compatibility effects in Parkinson's disease , 2011, Cortex.

[181]  M. Costantini,et al.  The space of affordances: A TMS study , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[182]  J. Witt Tool use influences perceived shape and perceived parallelism, which serve as indirect measures of perceived distance. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[183]  James R. Brockmole,et al.  Compressing perceived distance with remote tool-use: real, imagined, and remembered. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[184]  L. Wheaton,et al.  One hand, two objects: Emergence of affordance in contexts , 2012, Brain and Cognition.

[185]  F. Osiurak,et al.  Tool use and perceived distance: when unreachable becomes spontaneously reachable , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[186]  D. Bub,et al.  On the dynamics of action representations evoked by names of manipulable objects. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[187]  A. Cangelosi,et al.  Electrophysiological Examination of Embodiment in Vision and Action , 2012, Psychological science.

[188]  M. Grossman,et al.  Everyday Action Impairment in Parkinson's Disease Dementia , 2012, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[189]  F. Osiurak,et al.  Mechanical problem-solving strategies in left-brain damaged patients and apraxia of tool use , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[190]  S. Thompson-Schill,et al.  Manual Experience Shapes Object Representations , 2013, Psychological science.

[191]  F. Binkofski,et al.  Two action systems in the human brain , 2013, Brain and Language.

[192]  R. Proctor,et al.  Object-based correspondence effects for action-relevant and surface-property judgments with keypress responses: evidence for a basis in spatial coding , 2013, Psychological research.

[193]  S. Cappa,et al.  Neural representations of unfamiliar objects are modulated by sensorimotor experience , 2013, Cortex.

[194]  S. Grant,et al.  Binocular vision enhances a rapidly evolving affordance priming effect: Behavioural and TMS evidence , 2013, Brain and Cognition.

[195]  Gunnar Declerck,et al.  Why motor simulation cannot explain affordance perception , 2013, Adapt. Behav..

[196]  F. Osiurak Apraxia of tool use is not a matter of affordances , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[197]  T. Ziemke,et al.  Theories and computational models of affordance and mirror systems: An integrative review , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[198]  Tetsushi Nonaka,et al.  Motor Variability but Functional Specificity: The Case of a C4 Tetraplegic Mouth Calligrapher , 2013 .

[199]  F. Osiurak,et al.  Handing a tool to someone can take more time than using it , 2013, Cognition.

[200]  Steven A. Jax,et al.  Response interference between functional and structural object-related actions is increased in patients with ideomotor apraxia. , 2013, Journal of neuropsychology.

[201]  D. Mirman,et al.  Incidental and context-responsive activation of structure- and function-based action features during object identification. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[202]  F. Osiurak,et al.  Apraxia of tool use: More evidence for the technical reasoning hypothesis , 2013, Cortex.

[203]  F. Osiurak,et al.  Within reach but not so reachable: Obstacles matter in visual perception of distances , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[204]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Action relations affect affordance selection: Evidence from visuomotor responses to paired objects , 2014 .

[205]  Laurel J Buxbaum,et al.  Uncovering the architecture of action semantics. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[206]  G. Vingerhoets Contribution of the posterior parietal cortex in reaching, grasping, and using objects and tools , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[207]  D. Mirman,et al.  Abnormal dynamics of activation of object use information in apraxia: Evidence from eyetracking , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[208]  F. Osiurak What Neuropsychology Tells us About Human Tool Use? The Four Constraints Theory (4CT): Mechanics, Space, Time, and Effort , 2014, Neuropsychology Review.

[209]  François Osiurak,et al.  Getting a tool gives wings: overestimation of tool-related benefits in a motor imagery task and a decision task , 2014, Psychological research.

[210]  François Osiurak,et al.  What about mechanical knowledge?: Comment on "Action semantics: A unifying conceptual framework for the selective use of multimodal and modality-specific object knowledge" by van Elk, van Schie, and Bekkering. , 2014, Physics of life reviews.

[211]  A. Borghi Affordances and contextual flexibility: Comment on "Action semantics: A unifying conceptual framework for the selective use of multimodal and modality-specific object knowledge" by Michiel van Elk, Hein van Schie and Harold Bekkering. , 2014, Physics of life reviews.

[212]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Visual context modulates potentiation of grasp types during semantic object categorization , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[213]  Zissis Pappas Dissociating Simon and affordance compatibility effects: Silhouettes and photographs , 2014, Cognition.

[214]  Guy A. Orban,et al.  The neural basis of human tool use , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[215]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Action semantics: A unifying conceptual framework for the selective use of multimodal and modality-specific object knowledge. , 2014, Physics of life reviews.

[216]  F. Osiurak Tool use and manual actions: The human body as a means versus an end , 2014, Cortex.

[217]  François Osiurak,et al.  Tool use disorders after left brain damage , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[218]  Michiel van Elk,et al.  The left inferior parietal lobe represents stored hand-postures for object use and action prediction , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[219]  F. Osiurak,et al.  Pliers, not fingers: Tool-action effect in a motor intention paradigm , 2014, Cognition.

[220]  G. Goldenberg Apraxia – The cognitive side of motor control , 2013, Cortex.

[221]  Christine E. Watson,et al.  A distributed network critical for selecting among tool-directed actions , 2015, Cortex.

[222]  Dietmar Heinke,et al.  Implied actions between paired objects lead to affordance selection by inhibition. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[223]  F. Osiurak,et al.  A goal-based mechanism for delayed motor intention: considerations from motor skills, tool use and action memory , 2015, Psychological research.