Do observers exploit binocular disparity information in motor tasks within dynamic telepresence environments?
暂无分享,去创建一个
Mark F. Bradshaw | Andrew R. Willis | Simon J. Watt | Ian R. L. Davies | Kathleen M. Elliott | Andrew R. Willis | S. Watt | I. Davies | M. Bradshaw | K. M. Elliott
[1] M F Bradshaw,et al. The design of telepresence systems: the task-dependent use of binocular disparity and motion parallax. , 1999, International journal of cognitive ergonomics.
[2] M. Landy,et al. Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.
[3] A. Milner,et al. The perception and prehension of objects oriented in the depth plane , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.
[4] M. Jeannerod. The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements , 1990, Psychological Medicine.
[5] Simon J Watt,et al. Binocular cues are important in controlling the grasp but not the reach in natural prehension movements , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[6] K Tsutsui,et al. Neural coding of 3D features of objects for hand action in the parietal cortex of the monkey. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[7] A M Wing,et al. Assessing and reporting the accuracy of position measurements made with optical tracking systems. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.
[8] S. Jackson,et al. A Kinematic Analysis of Goal-directed Prehension Movements Executed under Binocular, Monocular, and Memory-guided Viewing Conditions , 1997 .
[9] S. Watt,et al. Binocular information in the control of prehensile movements in multiple-object scenes. , 2002, Spatial vision.
[10] F. Previc. Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in humans: Its ecological origins and neurophysiological implications , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[11] H. Sakata,et al. The TINS Lecture The parietal association cortex in depth perception and visual control of hand action , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.
[12] H. Sakata,et al. Selectivity of the parietal visual neurones in 3D orientation of surface of stereoscopic stimuli. , 1996, Neuroreport.
[13] J. M. Foley. Binocular distance perception. , 1980, Psychological review.
[14] Melvyn A. Goodale,et al. The role of binocular vision in prehension: a kinematic analysis , 1992, Vision Research.
[15] M F Bradshaw,et al. Cues to Viewing Distance for Stereoscopic Depth Constancy , 1998, Perception.
[16] D. Post,et al. A Study of Direct Distance Estimations to Familiar Objects in Real-Space, Two-Dimensional, and Stereographic Displays , 1982 .
[17] M. Jeannerod. The timing of natural prehension movements. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.
[18] M F Bradshaw,et al. Disparity Scaling and the Perception of Frontoparallel Surfaces , 1995, Perception.
[19] D. G. Green,et al. Monocular versus Binocular Visual Acuity , 1965, Nature.
[20] M. Mon-Williams,et al. The use of vergence information in the programming of prehension , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[21] Mark F. Bradshaw,et al. Can observers exploit enhanced-disparity information to control reaching movements within telepresence environments? , 2000, Electronic Imaging.
[22] Alan V. Oppenheim,et al. Discrete-time Signal Processing. Vol.2 , 2001 .