Apraxia Impairs Intentional Retrieval of Incidentally Acquired Motor Knowledge
暂无分享,去创建一个
Gereon R Fink | Peter H Weiss | Anna Dovern | Hans Karbe | Iring Koch | Jochen Saliger | P. Weiss | G. Fink | H. Karbe | I. Koch | A. Dovern | J. Saliger
[1] P. Matthews,et al. The role of ipsilateral premotor cortex in hand movement after stroke , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[2] C D Marsden,et al. Limb apraxias: higher-order disorders of sensorimotor integration. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[3] K. Heilman,et al. Acquisition and retention of gestures by apraxic patients , 1984, Brain and Cognition.
[4] Anne Springer,et al. Predicting and memorizing observed action: Differential premotor cortex involvement , 2011, Human brain mapping.
[5] J. Hermsdörfer,et al. Cortical Correlates of Gesture Processing: Clues to the Cerebral Mechanisms Underlying Apraxia during the Imitation of Meaningless Gestures , 2001, NeuroImage.
[6] J. Hermsdörfer,et al. Neural representations of pantomimed and actual tool use: Evidence from an event-related fMRI study , 2007, NeuroImage.
[7] R. Lyle. A performance test for assessment of upper limb function in physical rehabilitation treatment and research , 1981, International journal of rehabilitation research. Internationale Zeitschrift fur Rehabilitationsforschung. Revue internationale de recherches de readaptation.
[8] K. Lovblad,et al. Neuroanatomy of hemispatial neglect and its functional components: a study using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[9] G. Goldenberg. Defective imitation of gestures in patients with damage in the left or right hemispheres. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[10] Chris Rorden,et al. Improving Lesion-Symptom Mapping , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] Georg Goldenberg,et al. The Neural Basis of Imitation is Body Part Specific , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[12] Peder J. Johnson,et al. Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. , 1994 .
[13] P. Weiss,et al. [How to diagnose and treat limb apraxia]. , 2011, Fortschritte der Neurologie-Psychiatrie.
[14] J. Rankin. Cerebral Vascular Accidents in Patients over the Age of 60: II. Prognosis , 1957, Scottish medical journal.
[15] J. Hermsdörfer,et al. It takes the whole brain to make a cup of coffee: the neuropsychology of naturalistic actions involving technical devices , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[16] Yong Li,et al. Different left brain regions are essential for grasping a tool compared with its subsequent use , 2010, NeuroImage.
[17] J Hermsdörfer,et al. The effect of tactile feedback on pantomime of tool use in apraxia , 2004, Neurology.
[18] E. Renzi,et al. Ideational apraxia: A quantitative study , 1968 .
[19] F. Dick,et al. Voxel-based lesion–symptom mapping , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[20] E. Renzi,et al. The token test: A sensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. , 1962, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[21] Z. Dienes,et al. Implicit learning: Below the subjective threshold , 1997 .
[22] Mark Mühlau,et al. Left inferior parietal dominance in gesture imitation: an fMRI study , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[23] D. Harrington,et al. Motor sequencing with left hemisphere damage. Are some cognitive deficits specific to limb apraxia? , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[24] R. Masters,et al. Implicit sequence learning processes after unilateral stroke , 2007, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.
[25] L. Buxbaum,et al. Ideational apraxia and naturalistic action. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[26] K M Heilman,et al. Three-dimensional computergraphic analysis of apraxia. Neural representations of learned movement. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[27] N Motomura,et al. Motor learning in ideomotor apraxia. , 1989, The International journal of neuroscience.
[28] Mark Hallett,et al. Treatment of limb apraxia: moving forward to improved action. , 2008, American journal of physical medicine & rehabilitation.
[29] D. Filion,et al. Implicit learning of a motor skill after mild and moderate stroke , 2006, Clinical rehabilitation.
[30] P. Weiss,et al. Deficient sequencing of pantomimes in apraxia , 2008, Neurology.
[31] I. Koch,et al. Learning hierarchically structured action sequences is unaffected by prefrontal-cortex lesion , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[32] Kenichi Ohki,et al. Conversion of Working Memory to Motor Sequence in the Monkey Premotor Cortex , 2003, Science.
[33] M Eimer,et al. Explicit and implicit learning of event sequences: evidence from event-related brain potentials. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[34] R. E Passingham,et al. Cerebral dominance for action in the human brain: the selection of actions , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[35] N. Geschwind,et al. Defective motor learning in ideomotor apraxia , 1975, Neurology.
[36] R. Passingham,et al. Temporary interference in human lateral premotor cortex suggests dominance for the selection of movements. A study using transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[37] K M Heilman,et al. Spatial planning deficits in limb apraxia. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[38] E. Renzi,et al. Performance of Left Brain-Damaged Patients on Imitation of Single Movements and Motor Sequences. Frontal and Parietal-Injured Patients Compared , 1983, Cortex.
[39] Joachim Hoffmann,et al. Implicit learning of loosely defined structures , 1998 .
[40] Richard I. Ivry,et al. Attention and Structure in Sequence Learning , 2004 .
[41] Gereon R. Fink,et al. Neural basis of pantomiming the use of visually presented objects , 2004, NeuroImage.
[42] F Rösler,et al. Implicit and explicit learning of event sequences: evidence for distinct coding of perceptual and motor representations. , 2000, Acta psychologica.
[43] S. Keele,et al. The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. , 2003, Psychological review.
[44] Iring Koch,et al. The impact of response mode on implicit and explicit sequence learning. , 2002, Experimental psychology.
[45] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[46] G. Goldenberg. Pantomime of object use: a challenge to cerebral localization of cognitive function , 2003, NeuroImage.
[47] M. Nissen,et al. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.