Anosognosia for hemiplegia as a global deficit in motor awareness: Evidence from the non-paralysed limb

The current study adds to the growing empirical research into the mechanisms underlying unawareness of paralysis following stroke (anosognosia for hemiplegia or AHP) by investigating action awareness for the non-paralysed limb in a single AHP patient. Visual feedback representing patient GG's goal-directed reaching movements was either modified by a computer or left unperturbed. Unlike healthy and brain-damaged controls, GG was unable to detect computer-generated visual perturbations as large as 20°. GG also failed to report awareness of the large on-line corrective movements that he made when compensating (often unsuccessfully) for the visual perturbations. These results suggest that the motor comparators implicated in AHP are functioning, but not at optimum levels. Moreover, because the current findings reveal a deficit in awareness for reaches with the unimpaired limb, it is suggestive of common right hemisphere networks for motor awareness in both limbs and that AHP may be a global deficit in motor awareness as opposed to a specific lack of awareness for a particular motor deficit.

[1]  P. Haggard,et al.  Localising awareness of action with transcranial magnetic stimulation , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  A. Berti,et al.  Modular structure of awareness for sensorimotor disorders: Evidence from anosognosia for hemiplegia and anosognosia for hemianaesthesia , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  P. Thier,et al.  Misattributions of agency in schizophrenia are based on imprecise predictions about the sensory consequences of one's actions. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  M. Jehkonen,et al.  Anosognosia after stroke: assessment, occurrence, subtypes and impact on functional outcome reviewed , 2006, Acta neurologica Scandinavica.

[5]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Social cognitive neuroscience: where are we heading? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[7]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  A modular planar robotic manipulandum with end-point torque control , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[8]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. , 2000, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[9]  H. Karnath,et al.  Tight Link Between Our Sense of Limb Ownership and Self-Awareness of Actions , 2008, Stroke.

[10]  BernhardBaier,et al.  Tight Link Between Our Sense of Limb Ownership and Self-Awareness of Actions , 2008 .

[11]  R. Newport,et al.  THE RIGHT HEMISPHERE AND THE SENSE OF AGENCY: EMBODIMENT OF OTHERS AND MISATTRIBUTION OF SELF-GENERATED ACTION , 2010 .

[12]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The role of proprioception in action recognition , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.

[13]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  A. Fotopoulou,et al.  Anosognosia for Motor Impairment Following Left Brain Damage , 2009, Neuropsychology.

[15]  A. Fotopoulou,et al.  Motor awareness in anosognosia for hemiplegia: experiments at last! , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  M. Desmurget,et al.  An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  R. Newport,et al.  Misattribution of movement agency following right parietal TMS , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[18]  J. Marshall,et al.  The neural consequences of conflict between intention and the senses. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  Hans-Otto Karnath,et al.  Awareness of the Functioning of One's Own Limbs Mediated by the Insular Cortex? , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  N. Payette,et al.  Beyond the Brain: Embodied, Situated and Distributed Cognition , 2008 .

[21]  A. Berti,et al.  Anosognosia for hemiplegia, neglect dyslexia, and drawing neglect: Clinical findings and theoretical considerations , 1996, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[22]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Effect of Distorted Visual Feedback on the Sense of Agency , 2008, Behavioural neurology.

[23]  A. Berti,et al.  Understanding Motor Awareness Through Normal and Pathological Behavior , 2006 .

[24]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Bottom-up transfer of sensory-motor plasticity to recovery of spatial cognition: visuomotor adaptation and spatial neglect. , 2003, Progress in brain research.

[25]  P. Haggard,et al.  The role of motor intention in motor awareness: an experimental study on anosognosia for hemiplegia. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  A. Newen,et al.  Beyond the comparator model: A multifactorial two-step account of agency , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[27]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[28]  J. Decety,et al.  From the perception of action to the understanding of intention , 2001, Nature reviews. Neuroscience.

[29]  D. Wolpert Computational approaches to motor control , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Roger Newport,et al.  Self-denial and the role of intentions in the attribution of agency , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[31]  P. Jenkinson,et al.  Imagining the impossible: Motor representations in anosognosia for hemiplegia , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  Ian Nimmo-Smith,et al.  Anosognosia for Plegia: Specificity, Extension, Partiality and Disunity of Bodily Unawareness , 2004, Cortex.

[33]  G. Vallar,et al.  Shared Cortical Anatomy for Motor Awareness and Motor Control , 2005, Science.

[34]  V. Ramachandran Anosognosia in Parietal Lobe Syndrome , 1995, Consciousness and Cognition.

[35]  A. Berti,et al.  The Anatomy of Anosognosia for Hemiplegia: A Meta-Analysis , 2004, Cortex.

[36]  P. Garthwaite,et al.  Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Enhancing Visuomotor Adaptation by Reducing Error Signals: Single-step (Aware) versus Multiple-step (Unaware) Exposure to Wedge Prisms , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  A. Fotopoulou,et al.  Explicit and implicit anosognosia or upper limb motor impairment , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Spatio-Temporal Prediction Modulates the Perception of Self-Produced Stimuli , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  John C. Marshall,et al.  Spatial Compression in Visual Neglect: A Case Study , 1991, Cortex.

[41]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.