Students' overuse of proportionality on missing-value problems: how numbers may change solutions

Previous research has shown that when confronted with missingvalue word problems, primary school students strongly tend to use proportional solution approaches, even if these approaches are inappropriate. We investigated whether (besides the missingvalue formulation of word problems) the numbers appearing in word problems are part of the superficial cues that lead students to (over)use proportionality. A test containing proportional and nonproportional word problems was given to 508 4th, 5th, and 6th graders. Numbers in these problems were experimentally manipulated so that the internal and external ratios were either integer or not. For proportional problems, performances decreased as a result of noninteger ratios. For nonproportional problems, noninteger ratios led to a decrease in the overuse of proportionality; only in the case of additive nonproportional problems was this decrease accompanied by an increase in correct answers. This effect decreased from 4th grade to 6th grade. Implications of this study for research and practice are discussed.

[1]  A. Watson,et al.  Seeing an Exercise as a Single Mathematical Object: Using Variation to Structure Sense-Making , 2006 .

[2]  S. Zeger,et al.  Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models , 1986 .

[3]  H. Freudenthal Didactical Phenomenology of Mathematical Structures , 1983 .

[4]  Fou-Lai Lin,et al.  DIFFERENTIATION OF STUDENTS' REASONING ON LINEAR AND QUADRATIC GEOMETRIC NUMBER PATTERNS , 2004 .

[5]  Paul Cobb,et al.  Constructivist, emergent, and sociocultural perspectives in the context of developmental research , 1996 .

[6]  Richard Lesh,et al.  A Models and Modeling Perspective on Metacognitive Functioning in Everyday Situations Where Problem Solvers Develop Mathematical Constructs , 2003 .

[7]  Athanasios Gagatsis,et al.  Students Improper Proportional Reasoning: The Case of Area and Volume of Rectangular Figures. , 2004 .

[8]  A. Tversky,et al.  Subjective Probability: A Judgment of Representativeness , 1972 .

[9]  Kaye Stacey,et al.  Finding and using patterns in linear generalising problems , 1989 .

[10]  Kathleen Hart,et al.  Children's Understanding of Mathematics , 1989 .

[11]  G. Brousseau Theory of didactical situations in mathematics , 1997 .

[12]  K. Hart,et al.  Children's understanding of mathematics: 11-16 , 1981 .

[13]  Thomas R. Post,et al.  Learning and teaching ratio and proportion: Research implications , 1993 .

[14]  Richard Lesh,et al.  Beyond Constructivism: Models and Modeling Perspectives on Mathematics Problem Solving, Learning, and Teaching , 2003 .

[15]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Remedying secondary school students' illusion of linearity: A teaching experiment , 2004 .

[16]  Jan Wyndhamn,et al.  Word problems and mathematical reasoning—A study of children's mastery of reference and meaning in textual realities , 1997 .

[17]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  Not Everything Is Proportional: Effects of Age and Problem Type on Propensities for Overgeneralization , 2005 .

[18]  E. Corte,et al.  Realistic considerations in mathematical modeling of school arithmetic word problems , 1994 .

[19]  E. Fischbein,et al.  THE ROLE OF IMPLICIT MODELS IN SOLVING VERBAL PROBLEMS IN MULTIPLICATION AND DIVISION , 1985 .

[20]  F. Marton,et al.  Learning and Awareness , 1997 .

[21]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  Pupils' over-reliance on linearity: a scholastic effect? , 2007, The British journal of educational psychology.

[22]  Some aspects of children's ability to solve mathematical problems , 1983 .

[23]  E. Fischbein,et al.  The Evolution with Age of Probabilistic, Intuitively Based Misconceptions. , 1997 .

[24]  E. Fischbein,et al.  Intuitions and Schemata in Mathematical Reasoning , 1999 .

[25]  E. Corte,et al.  Making sense of word problems , 2000 .

[26]  Lesley Lee An Initiation into Algebraic Culture through Generalization Activities , 1996 .

[27]  H. Freudenthal Mathematics as an Educational Task , 1972 .

[28]  Thomas R. Post,et al.  Learning and teaching ratio and proportion: Research implications: Middle grades mathematics , 1993 .

[29]  Steven Pulos,et al.  Proportional reasoning: A review of the literature , 1985 .

[30]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Just Answering … or Thinking? Contrasting Pupils' Solutions and Classifications of Missing-Value Word Problems , 2010 .

[31]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Improper use of linear reasoning: An in-depth study of the nature and the irresistibility of secondary school students' errors , 2002 .

[32]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  The Linear Imperative: An Inventory and Conceptual Analysis of Students' Overuse of Linearity , 2008 .

[33]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Proportional reasoning as a heuristic-based process: time constraint and dual task considerations. , 2009, Experimental psychology.

[34]  Brian Greer,et al.  The Mathematical Modeling Perspective on Wor(l)d Problems. , 1993 .

[35]  E. Fischbein,et al.  Does the teaching of probability improve probabilistic intuitions? , 1984 .

[36]  Amy B. Ellis Connections Between Generalizing and Justifying: Students' Reasoning with Linear Relationships , 2007 .

[37]  Gary G. Koch,et al.  Categorical Data Analysis Using The SAS1 System , 1995 .

[38]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  The Illusion of Linearity: Expanding the evidence towards probabilistic reasoning , 2003 .

[39]  B. Greer Nonconservation of Multiplication and Division Involving Decimals. , 1987 .

[40]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  Do realistic contexts and graphical representations always have a beneficial impact on students' performance? Negative evidence from a study on modelling non-linear geometry problems , 2003 .

[41]  Brian Greer,et al.  Choice of operation in verbal arithmetic problems: the effects of number size, problem structure and context , 1984 .