Visual search without central vision – no single pseudofovea location is best

We typically fixate targets such that they are projected onto the fovea for best spatial resolution. Macular degeneration patients often develop fixation strategies such that targets are projected to an intact eccentric part of the retina, called pseudofovea. A longstanding debate concerns which pseudofovea-location is optimal for non-foveal vision. We examined how pseudofovea position and eccentricity affect performance in visual search, when vision is restricted to an off-foveal retinal region by a gaze-contingent display that dynamically blurs the stimulus except within a small viewing window (forced field location). Trained normally sighted participants were more accurate when forced field location was congruent with the required scan path direction; this contradicts the view that a single pseudofovea location is generally best. Rather, performance depends on the congruence between pseudofovea location and scan path direction.

[1]  S. Trauzettel-Klosinski,et al.  Eye movements in reading with hemianopic field defects: the significance of clinical parameters , 1998, Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology.

[2]  L. Arend,et al.  Reading with a macular scotoma. I. Retinal location of scotoma and fixation area. , 1986, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[3]  W. Schneider VAM: A neuro-cognitive model for visual attention control of segmentation, object recognition, and space-based motor action , 1995 .

[4]  Bartlett W. Mel,et al.  Contour Enhancement Benefits Older Adults with Simulated Central Field Loss , 2022 .

[5]  L Stark,et al.  Study of eccentric fixation with secondary visual feedback. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[6]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[7]  Robert W Massof,et al.  Is there a standard of care for eccentric viewing training? , 2004, Journal of rehabilitation research and development.

[8]  J. H. Bertera The effect of simulated scotomas on visual search in normal subjects. , 1988, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[9]  G. Legge,et al.  Psychophysics of reading—I. Normal vision , 1985, Vision Research.

[10]  Usha Chakravarthy,et al.  Age related macular degeneration , 2003, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[11]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Neuronal Basis of Covert Spatial Attention in the Frontal Eye Field , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  Psychophysics of reading—II. Low vision , 1985, Vision Research.

[13]  C. Frennesson,et al.  Location and Stability of a Newly Established Eccentric Retinal Locus Suitable for Reading, Achieved through Training of Patients with a Dense Central Scotoma , 1998, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[14]  Jens Schwarzbach,et al.  Adaptive strategies for reading with a forced retinal location. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[15]  Eric Castet,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of visual attention during saccade preparation: Independence and coupling between attention and movement planning. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[16]  Jeremy Nathans,et al.  Macular degeneration: recent advances and therapeutic opportunities , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  Usha Chakravarthy,et al.  Bevacizumab for the treatment of neovascular age related macular degeneration , 2010, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[18]  R. Walker,et al.  A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[19]  Jens Schwarzbach,et al.  (Un-) coupling gaze and attention outside central vision. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[20]  J. O'regan,et al.  Is there a systematic location for the pseudo-fovea in patients with central scotoma? , 1993, Vision Research.

[21]  M. Kalloniatis,et al.  Word Acuity Threshold as a Function of Contrast and Retinal Eccentricity , 2001, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[22]  R A Schuchard,et al.  Relative locations of macular scotomas near the PRL: effect on low vision reading. , 1999, Journal of rehabilitation research and development.

[23]  Gordon E Legge,et al.  Psychophysics of reading XX. Linking letter recognition to reading speed in central and peripheral vision , 2001, Vision Research.

[24]  P. Jong Prevalence of age-related macular degeneration in the United States. , 2004 .

[25]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  Rehabilitation of hemianopic dyslexia: no words necessary , 2009, Nature Clinical Practice Neurology.

[27]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  Robert W. Kentridge,et al.  Rehabilitation of hemianopic dyslexia: are words necessary for re-learning oculomotor control? , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  Xian Zhang,et al.  The spatial distribution of selective attention assessed using the multifocal visual evoked potential , 2002, Vision Research.

[30]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[31]  Benita J. O’Colmain,et al.  Prevalence of age-related macular degeneration in the United States. , 2004, Archives of ophthalmology.

[32]  K Rayner,et al.  Reading without a fovea. , 1979, Science.

[33]  K L Petre,et al.  Reading with Eccentric Fixation is Faster in Inferior Visual Field Than in Left Visual Field , 2000, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[34]  M. Zarbin,et al.  Age-Related Macular Degeneration: Review of Pathogenesis , 1998, European journal of ophthalmology.

[35]  C. Frennesson,et al.  Patients with AMD and a large absolute central scotoma can be trained successfully to use eccentric viewing, as demonstrated in a scanning laser ophthalmoscope , 2003, Vision Research.

[36]  Frans W. Cornelissen,et al.  The effect of visual field defects on eye movements and practical fitness to drive , 2002, Vision Research.

[37]  H. Bedell,et al.  The oculomotor reference in humans with bilateral macular disease. , 1990, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[38]  U. Leonards,et al.  Simulation of artificial vision: I. Eccentric reading of isolated words, and perceptual learning , 2003, Vision Research.

[39]  T. Schenk,et al.  The Premotor theory of attention: Time to move on? , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Timothy McMahon,et al.  Eye-movement training for reading in patients with age-related macular degeneration. , 2005, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[41]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Influence of Artificial Scotomas on Eye Movements during Visual Search , 2005, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[42]  J. Danckert Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX Wolfgang Prinz, Bernhard Hommel (Eds.), Oxford University Press, 2002, Price: £ 65.00, ISBN: 0-19-851069 , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  D. Sparks,et al.  Conceptual issues related to the role of the superior colliculus in the control of gaze , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[44]  Katherine M. Armstrong,et al.  Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex , 2003, Nature.

[45]  Bosco S. Tjan,et al.  Rapid and Persistent Adaptability of Human Oculomotor Control in Response to Simulated Central Vision Loss , 2013, Current Biology.