Selecting and ignoring salient objects within and across dimensions in visual search
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Martin Eimer,et al. Attentional capture by visual singletons is mediated by top-down task set: new evidence from the N2pc component. , 2008, Psychophysiology.
[2] Marvin M. Chun,et al. Making use of texton gradients: visual search and perceptual grouping exploit the same parallel processes in different ways , 1995 .
[3] H. J. Muller,et al. Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus- and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] H. Müller,et al. Searching for unknown feature targets on more than one dimension: Investigating a “dimension-weighting” account , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[5] Erich Schröger,et al. Visual distraction: a behavioral and event-related brain potential study in humans , 2006, Neuroreport.
[6] Erich Schrger,et al. A Neural Mechanism for Involuntary Attention Shifts to Changes in Auditory Stimulation , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[7] S. Luck,et al. Neural sources of focused attention in visual search. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.
[8] H G Vaughan,et al. The scalp topography of potentials in auditory and visual Go/NoGo tasks. , 1977, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[9] R. Näätänen. Attention and brain function , 1992 .
[10] Geoffrey F Woodman,et al. Serial deployment of attention during visual search. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] J. Theeuwes. Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[12] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[13] Geoffrey F. Woodman,et al. Serial deployment of attention during visual search. , 2003 .
[14] E. Donchin,et al. On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. , 1977, Psychophysiology.
[15] G. McCarthy,et al. Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.
[16] Andrew B. Leber,et al. Coordination of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Attentional Control in Human Cortex , 2005, Psychological science.
[17] J. Duncan,et al. Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.
[18] S J Luck,et al. Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] N. Lavie. Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] Jillian H. Fecteau,et al. Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target selection , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[21] J. Theeuwes. Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[22] S. Hochstein,et al. Task difficulty and the specificity of perceptual learning , 1997, Nature.
[23] B. Julesz. Textons, the elements of texture perception, and their interactions , 1981, Nature.
[24] C. Olivers,et al. Feature priming in visual search does not depend on the dimensional context , 2008 .
[25] S. Hillyard,et al. Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[26] J. Theeuwes,et al. Electrophysiological Evidence of the Capture of Visual Attention , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[27] Joseph Krummenacher,et al. Dimension- and space-based intertrial effects in visual pop-out search: modulation by task demands for focal-attentional processing , 2009, Psychological research.
[28] Piotr Jaskowski,et al. Evidence for an Integrative Role of P3b in Linking Reaction to Perception , 2005 .
[29] K. Cave,et al. Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[30] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. The Event Related Cortical Potential as an Index of Task Workload , 1977 .
[31] J. Driver,et al. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .
[32] J. C. Johnston,et al. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] Charles L. Folk,et al. Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings , 2008 .
[34] H. Nothdurft. Feature analysis and the role of similarity in preattentive vision , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[35] Kyle R Cave,et al. Roles of salience and strategy in conjunction search. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] C Meinecke,et al. Automaticity and attention: investigating automatic processing in texture segmentation with event-related brain potentials. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[37] Andrea M Philipp,et al. Effects of response selection on the task repetition benefit in task switching , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[38] H. Nothdurft. The role of features in preattentive vision: Comparison of orientation, motion and color cues , 1993, Vision Research.
[39] E. Vogel,et al. Electrophysiological Evidence for a Postperceptual Locus of Suppression during the Attentional Blink Time-based Attention and the Attentional Blink , 1998 .
[40] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[41] P. Jolicoeur,et al. The time course of the contingent spatial blink. , 2005, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[42] S. Yantis,et al. Visual Attention: Bottom-Up Versus Top-Down , 2004, Current Biology.
[43] James R. Bergen,et al. Parallel versus serial processing in rapid pattern discrimination , 1983, Nature.
[44] J. Theeuwes,et al. On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention , 2000 .
[45] H. J. Muller,et al. Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[46] H. Egeth,et al. Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[47] S. Hochstein,et al. How Serial is Serial Processing in Vision? , 1989, Perception.
[48] S A Hillyard,et al. Decision-related cortical potentials during an auditory signal detection task with cued observation intervals. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[49] Andrew B. Leber,et al. Made you blink! Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[50] S. Luck,et al. Bridging the Gap between Monkey Neurophysiology and Human Perception: An Ambiguity Resolution Theory of Visual Selective Attention , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.
[51] Wieske van Zoest,et al. Bottom-up and Top-down Control in Visual Search , 2004, Perception.
[52] H J Müller,et al. Visual search for singleton feature targets within and across feature dimensions , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[53] D. Alan Allport,et al. SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .
[54] H. Müller,et al. Attentional capture by salient color singleton distractors is modulated by top-down dimensional set. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[55] Frank E. Ritter,et al. The Rise of Cognitive Architectures , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.
[56] J. Wolfe,et al. Changing your mind: on the contributions of top-down and bottom-up guidance in visual search for feature singletons. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[58] C. Koch,et al. Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[59] Erich Schröger,et al. Attentional resources and pop-out detection in search displays , 2007, Neuroreport.
[60] S. Luck,et al. Attention to Features Precedes Attention to Locations in Visual Search: Evidence from Electromagnetic Brain Responses in Humans , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[61] J. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[62] J. Theeuwes. Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[63] S. Luck,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. , 1994, Psychophysiology.
[64] Martin Eimer,et al. Involuntary Attentional Capture is Determined by Task Set: Evidence from Event-related Brain Potentials , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[65] A. Treisman. Features and Objects: The Fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[66] J. Theeuwes. Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[67] Joseph Krummenacher,et al. Cross-trial priming of element positions in visual pop-out search is dependent on stimulus arrangement. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[68] Anna Schubö,et al. Salience detection and attentional capture , 2009, Psychological research.
[69] T. Goschke. Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching , 2000 .
[70] I. Koch,et al. The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[71] Eric Ruthruff,et al. Contingent attentional capture by top-down control settings: converging evidence from event-related potentials. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[72] E. Donchin,et al. Psychophysiology : systems, processes, and applications , 1987 .
[73] N. Lavie,et al. Frontal control of attentional capture in visual search , 2006 .
[74] J. Theeuwes,et al. Cuing the dimension of a distractor: Verbal cues of target identity also benefit same-dimension distractor singletons , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[75] Edmund Wascher,et al. Visual search strategies are indexed by event-related lateralizations of the EEG , 2003, Biological Psychology.
[76] R. Remington,et al. Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[77] Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 4.0: Current Progress With a Model of Visual Search , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.
[78] Pierre Jolicoeur,et al. Tracking the Location of Visuospatial Attention in a Contingent Capture Paradigm , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[79] C. Olivers,et al. On the dissociation between compound and present/absent tasks in visual search: Intertrial priming is ambiguity driven , 2006 .
[80] Dominique Lamy,et al. Attentional capture in singleton-detection and feature-search modes. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[81] Dietmar Heinke,et al. Working memory can guide pop-out search , 2006, Vision Research.
[82] Christian N L Olivers,et al. Attentional guidance by salient feature singletons depends on intertrial contingencies. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[83] Steven A. Hillyard,et al. Decision-related cortical potentials during an auditory signal detection task with cued observation intervals , 1975 .
[84] E. Donchin,et al. P300 and tracking difficulty: evidence for multiple resources in dual-task performance. , 1980, Psychophysiology.
[85] C. Bundesen. A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.
[86] Erich Schröger,et al. Texture segmentation and visual search for pop-out targets. An ERP study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[87] E. Amenedo,et al. MMN in the visual modality: a review , 2003, Biological Psychology.