Spatial updating in superimposed real and virtual environments

Wang (2004) showed that people do not always simultaneously update their relationships to real and imagined environments in a dual-environment situation. Employing the same paradigm, we examined whether spatial updating operates on virtual reality as it does on a real or fictitious environment. Participants learned target locations in a real room and a virtual kitchen. Then they turned to face targets either in the room or in the kitchen, while blindfolded, and pointed to the targets before and after turning. Participants kept track of their orientation in both environments equally efficiently, regardless of explicit instructions. In contrast, when the real environment was described verbally but not directly perceived, participants automatically updated the virtual kitchen but not the room. These results suggest that people automatically update a virtual environment as they do a real one when the two environments are superimposed. The automaticity of spatial updating is discussed.

[1]  Betsy Williams Sanders,et al.  Functional similarities in spatial representations between real and virtual environments , 2007, TAP.

[2]  A S Etienne,et al.  Path integration in mammals and its interaction with visual landmarks. , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[3]  T. S. Collett,et al.  How desert ants cope with enforced detours on their way home , 1992, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[4]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Visual cues can be sufficient for triggering automatic, reflexlike spatial updating , 2005, TAP.

[5]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Perceiving Real-World Viewpoint Changes , 1998 .

[6]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Spatial updating relies on an egocentric representation of space: Effects of the number of objects , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[7]  C C Presson,et al.  Updating after Rotational and Translational Body Movements: Coordinate Structure of Perspective Space , 1994, Perception.

[8]  Dennis Proffitt,et al.  Quantifying immersion in virtual reality , 1997, SIGGRAPH.

[9]  T. McNamara Mental representations of spatial relations , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  R. Wehner,et al.  The hidden spiral: systematic search and path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis , 1994, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[11]  Roy A. Ruddle,et al.  Navigating Large-Scale Virtual Environments: What Differences Occur Between Helmet-Mounted and Desk-Top Displays? , 1999, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[12]  Se-yuen Mak Easy to do , 1999 .

[13]  James R. Brockmole,et al.  Simultaneous spatial updating in nested environments , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[14]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Human navigation in nested environments. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[15]  Peter J. Werkhoven,et al.  The Effects of Proprioceptive and Visual Feedback on Geographical Orientation in Virtual Environments , 1999, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[16]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  Orientation specificity and spatial updating of memories for layouts. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  Thomas B. Sheridan,et al.  Musings on Telepresence and Virtual Presence , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[18]  J. Rieser Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Locomotion Mode Affects the Updating of Objects Encountered During Travel: The Contribution of Vestibular and Proprioceptive Inputs to Path Integration , 1998, Presence.

[20]  U. Paul Do Geese Use Path Integration for Walking Home , 1982 .

[21]  R. Wang Between reality and imagination: When is spatial updating automatic? , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al.  Spatial updating of virtual displays , 2004 .

[23]  Mary K. Kaiser,et al.  Perceived Orientation in Physical and Virtual Environments: Changes in Perceived Orientation as a Function of Idiothetic Information Available , 2002, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[24]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Spatial updating in real and virtual environments: contribution and interaction of visual and vestibular cues , 2004, APGV '04.

[25]  D. Proffitt,et al.  Spatial updating of virtual displays during self- and display rotation. , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[26]  Marios N. Avraamides,et al.  Spatial updating of environments described in texts , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[27]  R L Klatzky,et al.  Path integration while ignoring irrelevant movement. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[28]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Searching behaviour of desert ants, genusCataglyphis (Formicidae, Hymenoptera) , 2004, Journal of comparative physiology.

[29]  Carrie Heeter,et al.  Being There: The Subjective Experience of Presence , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[30]  Albert C. Stevens,et al.  Distortions in judged spatial relations , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  R. D. Easton,et al.  Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[32]  L. Regolin,et al.  Detour behaviour in the domestic chick: searching for a disappearing prey or a disappearing social partner , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[33]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Distal Attribution and Presence , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[34]  J. Jonides,et al.  Evidence of hierarchies in cognitive maps , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[35]  Stuart C. Grant,et al.  Contributions of Proprioception to Navigation in Virtual Environments , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[36]  J. Loomis,et al.  Immersive virtual environment technology as a basic research tool in psychology , 1999, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[37]  Sarah S. Chance,et al.  Spatial Updating of Self-Position and Orientation During Real, Imagined, and Virtual Locomotion , 1998 .

[38]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Active and passive scene recognition across views , 1999, Cognition.

[39]  R Wehner,et al.  Path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis. , 1988, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  A Berthoz,et al.  Updating an object’s orientation and location during nonvisual navigation: A comparison between two processing modes , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  H. Mittelstaedt,et al.  Homing by path integration in a mammal , 1980, Naturwissenschaften.

[42]  Steve Pettifer,et al.  Transfer of route learning from virtual to real environments. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[43]  J. Thomson,et al.  Automatic Spatial Updating during Locomotion without Vision , 1998, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[44]  M. de Vega,et al.  Updating spatial layouts mediated by pointing and labelling under physical and imaginary rotation , 2001 .

[45]  Martin J. Farrell,et al.  Mental Rotation and the Automatic Updating of Body-Centered Spatial Relationships , 1998 .

[46]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Spatial updating in virtual reality: the sufficiency of visual information , 2007, Psychological research.