Oxytocin facilitates reciprocity in social communication

Abstract Synchrony in social groups may confer significant evolutionary advantages by improving group cohesion and social interaction. However, the neurobiological mechanisms translating social synchrony into refined social information transmission between interacting individuals are still elusive. In two successively conducted experiments involving a total of 306 healthy volunteers, we explored the involvement of the neuropeptide oxytocin (OXT) in reciprocal social interaction. First, we show that synchronous social interactions evoke heightened endogenous OXT release in dyadic partners. In a second step, we examined the consequences of elevated OXT concentrations on emotion transmission by intranasally administering synthetic OXT before recording emotional expressions. Intriguingly, our data demonstrate that the subjects’ facial and vocal expressiveness of fear and happiness is enhanced after OXT compared with placebo administration. Collectively, our findings point to a central role of social synchrony in facilitating reciprocal communication between individuals via heightened OXT signaling. Elevated OXT concentrations among synchronized individuals seem to augment the partners’ emotional expressiveness, thereby contributing to improved transmission of emotional information in social communication.

[1]  Shihui Han,et al.  Oxytocin enhances inter-brain synchrony during social coordination in male adults. , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[2]  R. Bagby,et al.  The 20-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: IV. Reliability and factorial validity in different languages and cultures , 2003 .

[3]  P. Niedenthal,et al.  Social functionality of human emotion. , 2012, Annual review of psychology.

[4]  J. Korf,et al.  Individual differences in emotional expressivity predict oxytocin responses to cortisol administration: Relevance to breast cancer? , 2007, Biological Psychology.

[5]  R. Hurlemann,et al.  Dissecting the Role of Oxytocin in the Formation and Loss of Social Relationships , 2016, Biological Psychiatry.

[6]  W. Miltner,et al.  Recognition and evaluation of emotional prosody in individuals with generalized social phobia: a pilot study. , 2007, Behaviour research and therapy.

[7]  Jonathan Levy,et al.  Oxytocin selectively modulates brain response to stimuli probing social synchrony , 2016, NeuroImage.

[8]  L. D. Saedeleer,et al.  The influence of Oxytocin on automatic motor simulation , 2014, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[9]  P. Pattison,et al.  Emotion recognition via facial expression and affective prosody in schizophrenia: a methodological review. , 2002, Clinical psychology review.

[10]  F. Ramseyer,et al.  Nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: coordinated body movement reflects relationship quality and outcome. , 2011, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[11]  Peter Kirsch,et al.  Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[12]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  You are in sync with me: Neural correlates of interpersonal synchrony with a partner , 2014, Neuroscience.

[13]  J. Topál,et al.  Oxytocin and social pretreatment have similar effects on processing of negative emotional faces in healthy adult males , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[14]  H. Yamasue,et al.  Letter to the Editor Reply: Does Imitation Act as an Oxytocin Nebulizer in Autism Spectrum Disorder? , 2022 .

[15]  Ian B. Hickie,et al.  Intranasal Oxytocin Improves Emotion Recognition for Youth with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[16]  Florence Gaunet,et al.  Behavioural synchronization from an ethological perspective: Overview of its adaptive value , 2016, Adapt. Behav..

[17]  K. Kendrick,et al.  Oxytocin enhances brain reward system responses in men viewing the face of their female partner , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[18]  Jerrold S. Meyer,et al.  Inhaled oxytocin increases positive social behaviors in newborn macaques , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  C. Spielberger,et al.  Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory , 1970 .

[21]  Christophe Boone,et al.  The effect of oxytocin on cooperation in a prisoner's dilemma depends on the social context and a person's social value orientation. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[22]  B. Averbeck,et al.  Oxytocin by intranasal and intravenous routes reaches the cerebrospinal fluid in rhesus macaques: determination using a novel oxytocin assay , 2018, Molecular Psychiatry.

[23]  Frank J. Bernieri,et al.  Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher-student interactions , 1988 .

[24]  Ulrike Ehlert,et al.  Intranasal Oxytocin Increases Positive Communication and Reduces Cortisol Levels During Couple Conflict , 2009, Biological Psychiatry.

[25]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Is It What You Do, or When You Do It? The Roles of Contingency and Similarity in Pro-Social Effects of Imitation , 2013, Cogn. Sci..

[26]  R. Ebstein,et al.  Parental Oxytocin and Early Caregiving Jointly Shape Children’s Oxytocin Response and Social Reciprocity , 2013, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[27]  Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  When Two Become One: The Role of Oxytocin in Interpersonal Coordination and Cooperation , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  René Hurlemann,et al.  Hormonal contraceptives suppress oxytocin-induced brain reward responses to the partner's face. , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[29]  B. Averbeck,et al.  CSF and Blood Oxytocin Concentration Changes following Intranasal Delivery in Macaque , 2014, PloS one.

[30]  S. Herpertz,et al.  Oxytocin Improves “Mind-Reading” in Humans , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[31]  Mark H. Davis Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. , 1983 .

[32]  R. Baaren,et al.  Mimicry for money: Behavioral consequences of imitation , 2003 .

[33]  K. Kendrick,et al.  Oxytocin facilitates social approach behavior in women , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[34]  Andrew H Kemp,et al.  The Impact of a Single Administration of Intranasal Oxytocin on the Recognition of Basic Emotions in Humans: A Meta-Analysis , 2013, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[35]  T. Hendler,et al.  The brain basis of social synchrony. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[36]  Michael J. Hove,et al.  It's all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation , 2009 .

[37]  M. T. Motley,et al.  Facial expression of emotion: A comparison of posed expressions versus spontaneous expressions in an interpersonal communication setting , 1988 .

[38]  Kristin Prehn,et al.  Intranasal oxytocin enhances emotion recognition from dynamic facial expressions and leaves eye-gaze unaffected , 2012, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[39]  G. Johar,et al.  I'll Know What You're Like When I See How You Feel , 2009, Psychological science.

[40]  Angela Sirigu,et al.  Promoting social behavior with oxytocin in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[41]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/High-Functioning Autism, Malesand Females, Scientists and Mathematicians , 2001, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[42]  Giuseppe Pagnoni,et al.  Sex differences in the neural and behavioral response to intranasal oxytocin and vasopressin during human social interaction , 2014, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[43]  Aleksandr Kogan,et al.  Thin-slicing study of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and the evaluation and expression of the prosocial disposition , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  Orna Zagoory-Sharon,et al.  Oxytocin during the initial stages of romantic attachment: Relations to couples’ interactive reciprocity , 2012, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[45]  A. Guastella,et al.  Recommendations for the standardisation of oxytocin nasal administration and guidelines for its reporting in human research , 2013, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[46]  Scott S. Wiltermuth,et al.  Synchrony and Cooperation , 2009, Psychological science.

[47]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children☆☆☆ , 2010 .

[48]  神田 信彦 Beck Depression Inventory-IIについての一考察 , 2004 .

[49]  Qi Xuan,et al.  Synchrony in Social Groups and Its Benefits , 2013, Handbook of Human Computation.

[50]  U. Fischbacher,et al.  Oxytocin Shapes the Neural Circuitry of Trust and Trust Adaptation in Humans , 2008, Neuron.

[51]  Ulrike Ehlert,et al.  Sex-specific effects of intranasal oxytocin on autonomic nervous system and emotional responses to couple conflict. , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[52]  L. King Emotional Expression, Ambivalence Over Expression, and Marital Satisfaction , 1993 .

[53]  H. Scheich,et al.  The "Magdeburger Prosodie-Korpus , 2002 .

[54]  S. Koehne,et al.  Fostering Social Cognition through an Imitation- and Synchronization-Based Dance/Movement Intervention in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Controlled Proof-of-Concept Study , 2015, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics.

[55]  K. Kendrick,et al.  Opposing effects of oxytocin on moral judgment in males and females , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[56]  E. Kochs,et al.  Plasma Oxytocin and Vasopressin do not Predict Neuropeptide Concentrations in Human Cerebrospinal Fluid , 2013, Journal of neuroendocrinology.

[57]  D. Meyer,et al.  Supporting Online Material Materials and Methods Som Text Figs. S1 to S6 References Evidence for a Collective Intelligence Factor in the Performance of Human Groups , 2022 .

[58]  D. Sheehan,et al.  The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. , 1998, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[59]  L. Soorya,et al.  Intranasal oxytocin versus placebo in the treatment of adults with autism spectrum disorders: a randomized controlled trial , 2012, Molecular Autism.

[60]  Jürgen Dammers,et al.  Early sensory encoding of affective prosody: Neuromagnetic tomography of emotional category changes , 2010, NeuroImage.

[61]  Isabel Dziobek,et al.  Moving in and out of synchrony: A concept for a new intervention fostering empathy through interactional movement and dance , 2012 .

[62]  P. Niedenthal Embodying Emotion , 2007, Science.

[63]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Capuchin Monkeys Display Affiliation Toward Humans Who Imitate Them , 2009, Science.

[64]  N. Ambady,et al.  The effects of fear and anger facial expressions on approach- and avoidance-related behaviors. , 2005, Emotion.

[65]  Markus Heinrichs,et al.  Oxytocin and the social brain: Neural mechanisms and perspectives in human research , 2014, Brain Research.

[66]  R. Feldman PARENT–INFANT SYNCHRONY: A BIOBEHAVIORAL MODEL OF MUTUAL INFLUENCES IN THE FORMATION OF AFFILIATIVE BONDS , 2012 .

[67]  K. Kendrick,et al.  Elevated cerebrospinal fluid and blood concentrations of oxytocin following its intranasal administration in humans , 2013, Scientific Reports.

[68]  J. Nadel,et al.  Children with Autism Display more Social Behaviors after Repeated Imitation Sessions , 2001, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[69]  Chris Moore,et al.  Are there Nonverbal Cues to Commitment? An Exploratory Study Using the Zero-Acquaintance Video Presentation Paradigm , 2003 .

[70]  Oxytocin and vasopressin flatten dominance hierarchy and enhance behavioral synchrony in part via anterior cingulate cortex , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[71]  A. Guastella,et al.  The Role of Oxytocin in Human Affect , 2011 .

[72]  K. Kendrick,et al.  Oxytocin Modulates Social Distance between Males and Females , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[73]  U. Fischbacher,et al.  Oxytocin increases trust in humans , 2005, Nature.