Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) display limited behavioural flexibility when faced with a changing foraging task requiring tool use
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] E. Ottoni,et al. The effects of tradition on problem solving by two wild populations of bearded capuchin monkeys in a probing task , 2016, Biology Letters.
[2] S. R. Ross,et al. Early Rearing Conditions and Captive Chimpanzee Behavior: Some Surprising Findings , 2006 .
[3] M. Bloomsmith,et al. Factors determining tool-using ability in two captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) colonies , 1995, Primates.
[4] Nicholas J. Mulcahy,et al. How great apes perform on a modified trap-tube task , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[5] H. Schielzeth,et al. Conclusions beyond support: overconfident estimates in mixed models , 2008, Behavioral ecology : official journal of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology.
[6] M. Tomasello,et al. Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[7] D. M. Fragaszy,et al. The fourth dimension of tool use: temporally enduring artefacts aid primates learning to use tools , 2013, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[8] Sarah J. Davis,et al. Foundations of cumulative culture in apes: improved foraging efficiency through relinquishing and combining witnessed behaviours in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[9] K. Zuberbühler,et al. Wild Chimpanzees Rely on Cultural Knowledge to Solve an Experimental Honey Acquisition Task , 2009, Current Biology.
[10] N. Morimura,et al. Effects of early rearing conditions on problem‐solving skill in captive male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2010, American journal of primatology.
[11] A. Whiten,et al. Prestige Affects Cultural Learning in Chimpanzees , 2010, PloS one.
[12] T. Gruber. Great Apes Do Not Learn Novel Tool Use Easily: Conservatism, Functional Fixedness, or Cultural Influence? , 2016, International Journal of Primatology.
[13] Andrew Whiten,et al. Social Learning and Culture in Child and Chimpanzee. , 2017, Annual review of psychology.
[14] Russell D. Gray,et al. The development of pandanus tool manufacture in wild New Caledonian crows , 2010 .
[15] T. Matsuzawa,et al. Invention and modification of a new tool use behavior: ant‐fishing in trees by a wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) at Bossou, Guinea , 2008, American journal of primatology.
[16] Sarah J. Davis,et al. Acquisition of a socially learned tool use sequence in chimpanzees: Implications for cumulative culture. , 2017, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.
[17] J. Call,et al. The cognitive underpinnings of flexible tool use in great apes. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal learning and cognition.
[18] J. Call,et al. Repeated innovation in great apes , 2013, Animal Behaviour.
[19] C. Schaik,et al. Skill mastery inhibits adoption of observed alternative solutions among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[20] L. Lefebvre,et al. Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds , 1997, Animal Behaviour.
[21] V. Reynolds,et al. Community-specific evaluation of tool affordances in wild chimpanzees , 2011, Scientific reports.
[22] Flexibility and Persistence of Chimpanzee (Pan Troglodytes) Foraging Behavior in a Captive Environmentxht , 2012, American journal of primatology.
[23] A. S. Griffin,et al. Tracking Changing Environments: Innovators Are Fast, but Not Flexible Learners , 2013, PloS one.
[24] Lydia M. Hopper,et al. Psychological limits on animal innovation , 2014, Animal Behaviour.
[25] Corina J. Logan,et al. How far will a behaviourally flexible invasive bird go to innovate? , 2016, Royal Society Open Science.
[26] J. Templeton,et al. Sociality and social learning in two species of corvids: the pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) and the Clark's nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana). , 1999, Journal of comparative psychology.
[27] J. Koolhaas,et al. Routine Formation and Flexibility in Social and Non-Social Behaviour of Aggressive and Non-Aggressive Male Mice , 1990 .
[28] W. Hoppitt,et al. Social Network Analysis Shows Direct Evidence for Social Transmission of Tool Use in Wild Chimpanzees , 2014, PLoS biology.
[29] Basis for Cumulative Cultural Evolution in Chimpanzees: Social Learning of a More Efficient Tool-Use Technique , 2013, PloS one.
[30] R Core Team,et al. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .
[31] J. Leca,et al. Indirect social influence in the maintenance of the stone-handling tradition in Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata , 2010, Animal Behaviour.
[32] M. Charbonneau. All Innovations are Equal, but Some More than Others: (Re)integrating Modification Processes to the Origins of Cumulative Culture , 2015 .
[33] Simon W. Townsend,et al. Vocal Learning in the Functionally Referential Food Grunts of Chimpanzees , 2015, Current Biology.
[34] Stephan R Lehner,et al. Can captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) be coaxed into cumulative build-up of techniques? , 2011, Journal of comparative psychology.
[35] Andrew Whiten,et al. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and the question of cumulative culture: an experimental approach , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[36] Lydia M. Hopper,et al. The importance of witnessed agency in chimpanzee social learning of tool use , 2015, Behavioural Processes.
[37] J. E. Bolhuis,et al. Individual coping characteristics, rearing conditions and behavioural flexibility in pigs , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.
[38] C. Boesch. Is culture a golden barrier between human and chimpanzee? , 2003 .
[39] C. Rutz,et al. Hook tool manufacture in New Caledonian crows: behavioural variation and the influence of raw materials , 2015, BMC Biology.
[40] Alan C Kamil,et al. Serial reversal learning and the evolution of behavioral flexibility in three species of North American corvids (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus, Nucifraga columbiana, Aphelocoma californica). , 2007, Journal of comparative psychology.
[41] Brian Hare,et al. Psychological Health of Orphan Bonobos and Chimpanzees in African Sanctuaries , 2011, PloS one.
[42] T. Jaeger,et al. Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.
[43] J. Call,et al. Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2009, Biology Letters.
[44] Elizabeth A. Hobson,et al. Behavioral flexibility and species invasions: the adaptive flexibility hypothesis , 2010 .
[45] M. Tomasello,et al. Humans Have Evolved Specialized Skills of Social Cognition: The Cultural Intelligence Hypothesis , 2007, Science.
[46] A. Kacelnik,et al. Flexibility in Problem Solving and Tool Use of Kea and New Caledonian Crows in a Multi Access Box Paradigm , 2011, PloS one.
[47] K. Laland. Social learning strategies , 2004, Learning & behavior.
[48] Lydia M. Hopper,et al. Experimental studies of traditions and underlying transmission processes in chimpanzees , 2007, Animal Behaviour.
[49] L. Lefebvre,et al. Brains, Innovations and Evolution in Birds and Primates , 2004, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.
[50] Daniel Sol,et al. Behavioural flexibility and invasion success in birds , 2002, Animal Behaviour.
[51] Lydia M. Hopper,et al. Chimpanzees copy dominant and knowledgeable individuals: implications for cultural diversity. , 2015, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.
[52] K. Slocombe,et al. Is music enriching for group-housed captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)? , 2017, PloS one.
[53] Kevin N. Laland,et al. Do animals have culture? , 2003 .
[54] Elaine N Videan,et al. Bed‐building in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the importance of early rearing , 2006, American journal of primatology.
[55] Mollie E. Brooks,et al. Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. , 2009, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[56] L. Lefebvre,et al. A field test of behavioural flexibility in Zenaida doves (Zenaida aurita) , 2010, Behavioural Processes.
[57] Kevin N Laland,et al. Transmission fidelity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[58] J. Call,et al. Comparing the Performances of Apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus) and Human Children (Homo sapiens) in the Floating Peanut Task , 2011, PloS one.
[59] Josep Call,et al. Great apes' (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus) understanding of tool functional properties after limited experience. , 2008, Journal of comparative psychology.
[60] Josep Call,et al. Reaching around barriers: the performance of the great apes and 3–5-year-old children , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[61] C. Rutz,et al. Tool bending in New Caledonian crows , 2016, Royal Society Open Science.
[62] J. Call,et al. Chimpanzees solve the trap problem when the confound of tool-use is removed. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[63] Michael J. Kuba,et al. Pull or Push? Octopuses Solve a Puzzle Problem , 2016, PloS one.
[64] Lydia M Hopper,et al. Observational learning in chimpanzees and children studied through ‘ghost’ conditions , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[65] A. Whiten,et al. Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees , 2005, Nature.
[66] A. Whiten,et al. Network Analysis of Social Changes in a Captive Chimpanzee Community Following the Successful Integration of Two Adult Groups , 2013, American journal of primatology.
[67] J. Call,et al. Age-dependent cognitive inflexibility in great apes , 2015, Animal Behaviour.
[68] K. Summers,et al. Learning to learn: advanced behavioural flexibility in a poison frog , 2016, Animal Behaviour.
[69] Louis Lefebvre,et al. What’s flexible in behavioral flexibility? , 2017 .
[70] S. Lea,et al. How practice makes perfect: the role of persistence, flexibility and learning in problem-solving efficiency , 2016, Animal Behaviour.
[71] A. Whiten,et al. A potent effect of observational learning on chimpanzee tool construction , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[72] J. Massen,et al. A behavioral view on chimpanzee personality: Exploration tendency, persistence, boldness, and tool‐orientation measured with group experiments , 2013, American journal of primatology.
[73] Lisa A. Reamer,et al. Socially transmitted diffusion of a novel behavior from subordinate chimpanzees , 2017, American journal of primatology.
[74] A. S. Griffin,et al. Innovation and problem solving: A review of common mechanisms , 2014, Behavioural Processes.
[75] K. Laland,et al. Primate Innovation: Sex, Age and Social Rank Differences , 2001, International Journal of Primatology.
[76] Kevin N Laland,et al. Human cumulative culture: a comparative perspective , 2014, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[77] T. Matsuzawa,et al. Acquisition and Transmission of Tool Making and Use for Drinking Juice in a Group of Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 1997 .
[78] C. Boesch. What makes us human (Homo sapiens)? The challenge of cognitive cross-species comparison. , 2007, Journal of comparative psychology.
[79] C. Rutz,et al. Strong between‐site variation in New Caledonian crows’ use of hook‐tool‐making materials , 2016, Biological journal of the Linnean Society. Linnean Society of London.
[80] Kathleen M. Silva,et al. Methodological-conceptual problems in the study of chimpanzees’ folk physics: How studies with adult humans can help , 2005, Learning & behavior.
[82] Stephen Jay Gould,et al. Is culture a golden barrier between human and chimpanzee? , 2003 .