Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) display limited behavioural flexibility when faced with a changing foraging task requiring tool use

Behavioural flexibility, the ability to alter behaviour in response to environmental feedback, and to relinquish previously successful solutions to problems, is a crucial ability in allowing organisms to adapt to novel environments and environmental change; it is essential to cumulative cultural change. To explore this ability in chimpanzees, 18 individuals (Pan troglodytes) were presented with an artificial foraging task consisting of a tube partially filled with juice that could be reached by hand or retrieved using tool materials to hand. Effective solutions were then restricted in the second phase of the study by narrowing the diameter of the tube, necessitating the abandonment of previously successful solutions. Chimpanzees showed limited behavioural flexibility in comparison to some previous studies, increasing their use of effective techniques, but also continuing to attempt solutions that had been rendered ineffective. This adds to a literature reporting divergent evidence for flexibility (the ability to alter behaviour in response to environmental feedback, and to relinquish previously successful solutions to problems) versus conservatism (a reluctance or inability to explore or adopt novel solutions to problems when a solution is already known) in apes.

[1]  E. Ottoni,et al.  The effects of tradition on problem solving by two wild populations of bearded capuchin monkeys in a probing task , 2016, Biology Letters.

[2]  S. R. Ross,et al.  Early Rearing Conditions and Captive Chimpanzee Behavior: Some Surprising Findings , 2006 .

[3]  M. Bloomsmith,et al.  Factors determining tool-using ability in two captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) colonies , 1995, Primates.

[4]  Nicholas J. Mulcahy,et al.  How great apes perform on a modified trap-tube task , 2006, Animal Cognition.

[5]  H. Schielzeth,et al.  Conclusions beyond support: overconfident estimates in mixed models , 2008, Behavioral ecology : official journal of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology.

[6]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  D. M. Fragaszy,et al.  The fourth dimension of tool use: temporally enduring artefacts aid primates learning to use tools , 2013, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  Sarah J. Davis,et al.  Foundations of cumulative culture in apes: improved foraging efficiency through relinquishing and combining witnessed behaviours in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[9]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Wild Chimpanzees Rely on Cultural Knowledge to Solve an Experimental Honey Acquisition Task , 2009, Current Biology.

[10]  N. Morimura,et al.  Effects of early rearing conditions on problem‐solving skill in captive male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2010, American journal of primatology.

[11]  A. Whiten,et al.  Prestige Affects Cultural Learning in Chimpanzees , 2010, PloS one.

[12]  T. Gruber Great Apes Do Not Learn Novel Tool Use Easily: Conservatism, Functional Fixedness, or Cultural Influence? , 2016, International Journal of Primatology.

[13]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Social Learning and Culture in Child and Chimpanzee. , 2017, Annual review of psychology.

[14]  Russell D. Gray,et al.  The development of pandanus tool manufacture in wild New Caledonian crows , 2010 .

[15]  T. Matsuzawa,et al.  Invention and modification of a new tool use behavior: ant‐fishing in trees by a wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) at Bossou, Guinea , 2008, American journal of primatology.

[16]  Sarah J. Davis,et al.  Acquisition of a socially learned tool use sequence in chimpanzees: Implications for cumulative culture. , 2017, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.

[17]  J. Call,et al.  The cognitive underpinnings of flexible tool use in great apes. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal learning and cognition.

[18]  J. Call,et al.  Repeated innovation in great apes , 2013, Animal Behaviour.

[19]  C. Schaik,et al.  Skill mastery inhibits adoption of observed alternative solutions among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2009, Animal Cognition.

[20]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[21]  V. Reynolds,et al.  Community-specific evaluation of tool affordances in wild chimpanzees , 2011, Scientific reports.

[22]  Flexibility and Persistence of Chimpanzee (Pan Troglodytes) Foraging Behavior in a Captive Environmentxht , 2012, American journal of primatology.

[23]  A. S. Griffin,et al.  Tracking Changing Environments: Innovators Are Fast, but Not Flexible Learners , 2013, PloS one.

[24]  Lydia M. Hopper,et al.  Psychological limits on animal innovation , 2014, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  Corina J. Logan,et al.  How far will a behaviourally flexible invasive bird go to innovate? , 2016, Royal Society Open Science.

[26]  J. Templeton,et al.  Sociality and social learning in two species of corvids: the pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) and the Clark's nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana). , 1999, Journal of comparative psychology.

[27]  J. Koolhaas,et al.  Routine Formation and Flexibility in Social and Non-Social Behaviour of Aggressive and Non-Aggressive Male Mice , 1990 .

[28]  W. Hoppitt,et al.  Social Network Analysis Shows Direct Evidence for Social Transmission of Tool Use in Wild Chimpanzees , 2014, PLoS biology.

[29]  Basis for Cumulative Cultural Evolution in Chimpanzees: Social Learning of a More Efficient Tool-Use Technique , 2013, PloS one.

[30]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[31]  J. Leca,et al.  Indirect social influence in the maintenance of the stone-handling tradition in Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[32]  M. Charbonneau All Innovations are Equal, but Some More than Others: (Re)integrating Modification Processes to the Origins of Cumulative Culture , 2015 .

[33]  Simon W. Townsend,et al.  Vocal Learning in the Functionally Referential Food Grunts of Chimpanzees , 2015, Current Biology.

[34]  Stephan R Lehner,et al.  Can captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) be coaxed into cumulative build-up of techniques? , 2011, Journal of comparative psychology.

[35]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and the question of cumulative culture: an experimental approach , 2008, Animal Cognition.

[36]  Lydia M. Hopper,et al.  The importance of witnessed agency in chimpanzee social learning of tool use , 2015, Behavioural Processes.

[37]  J. E. Bolhuis,et al.  Individual coping characteristics, rearing conditions and behavioural flexibility in pigs , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.

[38]  C. Boesch Is culture a golden barrier between human and chimpanzee? , 2003 .

[39]  C. Rutz,et al.  Hook tool manufacture in New Caledonian crows: behavioural variation and the influence of raw materials , 2015, BMC Biology.

[40]  Alan C Kamil,et al.  Serial reversal learning and the evolution of behavioral flexibility in three species of North American corvids (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus, Nucifraga columbiana, Aphelocoma californica). , 2007, Journal of comparative psychology.

[41]  Brian Hare,et al.  Psychological Health of Orphan Bonobos and Chimpanzees in African Sanctuaries , 2011, PloS one.

[42]  T. Jaeger,et al.  Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.

[43]  J. Call,et al.  Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2009, Biology Letters.

[44]  Elizabeth A. Hobson,et al.  Behavioral flexibility and species invasions: the adaptive flexibility hypothesis , 2010 .

[45]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Humans Have Evolved Specialized Skills of Social Cognition: The Cultural Intelligence Hypothesis , 2007, Science.

[46]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Flexibility in Problem Solving and Tool Use of Kea and New Caledonian Crows in a Multi Access Box Paradigm , 2011, PloS one.

[47]  K. Laland Social learning strategies , 2004, Learning & behavior.

[48]  Lydia M. Hopper,et al.  Experimental studies of traditions and underlying transmission processes in chimpanzees , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[49]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  Brains, Innovations and Evolution in Birds and Primates , 2004, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[50]  Daniel Sol,et al.  Behavioural flexibility and invasion success in birds , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[51]  Lydia M. Hopper,et al.  Chimpanzees copy dominant and knowledgeable individuals: implications for cultural diversity. , 2015, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.

[52]  K. Slocombe,et al.  Is music enriching for group-housed captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)? , 2017, PloS one.

[53]  Kevin N. Laland,et al.  Do animals have culture? , 2003 .

[54]  Elaine N Videan,et al.  Bed‐building in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the importance of early rearing , 2006, American journal of primatology.

[55]  Mollie E. Brooks,et al.  Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. , 2009, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[56]  L. Lefebvre,et al.  A field test of behavioural flexibility in Zenaida doves (Zenaida aurita) , 2010, Behavioural Processes.

[57]  Kevin N Laland,et al.  Transmission fidelity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[58]  J. Call,et al.  Comparing the Performances of Apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus) and Human Children (Homo sapiens) in the Floating Peanut Task , 2011, PloS one.

[59]  Josep Call,et al.  Great apes' (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus) understanding of tool functional properties after limited experience. , 2008, Journal of comparative psychology.

[60]  Josep Call,et al.  Reaching around barriers: the performance of the great apes and 3–5-year-old children , 2009, Animal Cognition.

[61]  C. Rutz,et al.  Tool bending in New Caledonian crows , 2016, Royal Society Open Science.

[62]  J. Call,et al.  Chimpanzees solve the trap problem when the confound of tool-use is removed. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[63]  Michael J. Kuba,et al.  Pull or Push? Octopuses Solve a Puzzle Problem , 2016, PloS one.

[64]  Lydia M Hopper,et al.  Observational learning in chimpanzees and children studied through ‘ghost’ conditions , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[65]  A. Whiten,et al.  Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees , 2005, Nature.

[66]  A. Whiten,et al.  Network Analysis of Social Changes in a Captive Chimpanzee Community Following the Successful Integration of Two Adult Groups , 2013, American journal of primatology.

[67]  J. Call,et al.  Age-dependent cognitive inflexibility in great apes , 2015, Animal Behaviour.

[68]  K. Summers,et al.  Learning to learn: advanced behavioural flexibility in a poison frog , 2016, Animal Behaviour.

[69]  Louis Lefebvre,et al.  What’s flexible in behavioral flexibility? , 2017 .

[70]  S. Lea,et al.  How practice makes perfect: the role of persistence, flexibility and learning in problem-solving efficiency , 2016, Animal Behaviour.

[71]  A. Whiten,et al.  A potent effect of observational learning on chimpanzee tool construction , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[72]  J. Massen,et al.  A behavioral view on chimpanzee personality: Exploration tendency, persistence, boldness, and tool‐orientation measured with group experiments , 2013, American journal of primatology.

[73]  Lisa A. Reamer,et al.  Socially transmitted diffusion of a novel behavior from subordinate chimpanzees , 2017, American journal of primatology.

[74]  A. S. Griffin,et al.  Innovation and problem solving: A review of common mechanisms , 2014, Behavioural Processes.

[75]  K. Laland,et al.  Primate Innovation: Sex, Age and Social Rank Differences , 2001, International Journal of Primatology.

[76]  Kevin N Laland,et al.  Human cumulative culture: a comparative perspective , 2014, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[77]  T. Matsuzawa,et al.  Acquisition and Transmission of Tool Making and Use for Drinking Juice in a Group of Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 1997 .

[78]  C. Boesch What makes us human (Homo sapiens)? The challenge of cognitive cross-species comparison. , 2007, Journal of comparative psychology.

[79]  C. Rutz,et al.  Strong between‐site variation in New Caledonian crows’ use of hook‐tool‐making materials , 2016, Biological journal of the Linnean Society. Linnean Society of London.

[80]  Kathleen M. Silva,et al.  Methodological-conceptual problems in the study of chimpanzees’ folk physics: How studies with adult humans can help , 2005, Learning & behavior.

[81]  Sociality and social learning in two species of corvids: the pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) and the Clark's nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana). , 1999 .

[82]  Stephen Jay Gould,et al.  Is culture a golden barrier between human and chimpanzee? , 2003 .