Likeness and Likelihood in Everyday Thought: Magical Thinking in Judgments About Personality [and Comments and Reply]

Magical thinking has perplexed anthropological theorists for nearly a century. At least three perspectives are extant: (1) Magic is a form of science, a relatively effective set of canons and procedures for acquiring knowledge and exercising control (see, e. g., Levi-Strauss 1966); (2) magic is a form of fantasy, an irrational symbolic attempt to influence uncontrollable events (see, e.g., Malinowski 1954); (3) magic is a form of rhetoric, a persuasive communication designed to arouse sentiments rather than make truth claims about what goes with what in experience (see, e.g., Tambiah 1973). This study presents an alternative, cognitive-processing, account of magical thinking. Magical thinking is an expansion of a universal disinclination of normal adults to draw correlational lessons from their experiences. Correlation and contingency are relatively complex concepts that are not spontaneously available to human thought and are not to be found in the reasoning of most normal adults in all cultures. In the absence of a concept of correlation or contingency normal adults often rely on the more intuitive notions of likeness and resemblance to estimate what goes with what in their experience. They collapse the distinction between likeness and co-occurrence likelihood; they merge the notion of resemblance and the notion of contingency. Magical thinking is a universal characteristic of everyday thought. It is no more distinctive of Zande beliefs that ringworm and fowl excrement go together than, for example, of our own beliefs about what goes with what in personality. In both cases, objects and events that resemble one another are thought to go together or influence oneanother. In both cases, "propositions about language" are confused with "propositions about the world" (D'Andrade 1965). The study presents the results of a series of cognitive experiments supportive of these claims and addresses a number of seminal questions in the "anthropology of though," viz., (1) What does the organization of symbolic systems tell us about the predilections and disinclinations of the human mind? (2) In what ways do symbolic systems influence the intellectual processes of those who use them? (3) How do objects and events give rise to meanings? (4) How are symbolic systems to be described? The study focuses upon the symbolization of individual differences inconduct, i.e., everyday personality theory. One theme of the study is that, for the most part, personality traits cannot be discovered in behavior, but are rather the creations of the magical mind. A second theme of the study is that magical thinking is inductive in its intent but mistaken in its conclusions, a view that some may find reminiscent of such late 19th-century theorists as E. B.Tylor and J. G. Frazer.

[1]  R. Rescorla Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[2]  D. Price-Williams A study concerning concepts of conservation of quantities among primitive children , 1961 .

[3]  H. M. Jenkins,et al.  The display of information and the judgment of contingency. , 1965, Canadian journal of psychology.

[4]  H. M. Jenkins,et al.  JUDGMENT OF CONTINGENCY BETWEEN RESPONSES AND OUTCOMES. , 1965, Psychological monographs.

[5]  P. Dasen Cross-Cultural Piagetian Research: A Summary , 1972 .

[6]  Richard A. Shweder,et al.  How relevant is an individual difference theory of personality? , 1975, Journal of personality.

[7]  J. Mehler,et al.  Cognitive Capacity of Very Young Children , 1967, Science.

[8]  W. Mischel,et al.  Maintaining trait consistency in the resolution of behavioral inconsistency: The wolf in sheep's clothing? , 1976 .

[9]  F. Bartlett Witchcraft, Oracles and Magic among the Azande , 1937, Nature.

[10]  E. Ramsden Biological Boundaries of Learning , 1973 .

[11]  H. Simon,et al.  Models Of Man : Social And Rational , 1957 .

[12]  Richard A. Shweder,et al.  Dream Concepts of Hausa Children , 1975 .

[13]  Harvey Pinney The Structure of Social Action , 1940, Ethics.

[14]  A. Dittmann,et al.  Person, Setting, and Change in Social Interaction , 1959 .

[15]  Andrew G. Walker The Reality of Ethnomethodology , 1977 .

[16]  John Garcia,et al.  Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning , 1966 .

[17]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability , 1973 .

[18]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. , 1977 .

[19]  J. Kemeny,et al.  Introduction to finite mathematics , 1957 .

[20]  E. E. Jones,et al.  The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. , 1972 .

[21]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  The Sciences of the Artificial , 1970 .

[22]  Patricia McBroom,et al.  Children of Six Cultures , 1968 .

[23]  Miriam Frances Dunn,et al.  The Psychology of Reasoning , 1900, Nature.

[24]  Allen Newell,et al.  Human Problem Solving. , 1973 .

[25]  W. Mischel Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality. , 1973, Psychological review.

[26]  P. Kay Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution , 1969 .

[27]  S Toulmin,et al.  Logic and the theory of mind. , 1975, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[28]  W. T. Norman,et al.  A universal conception of personality structure? , 1966, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  R. Burton Validity of retrospective reports assessed by the multitrait-multimethod analysis. , 1970 .

[30]  H. Ross Principles of Numerical Taxonomy , 1964 .

[31]  W. T. Norman,et al.  Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes: replicated factors structure in peer nomination personality ratings. , 1963, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[32]  D. W. Fiske The limits for the conventional science of personality. , 1974, Journal of personality.

[33]  D. Campbell,et al.  Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. , 1959, Psychological bulletin.

[34]  R. Horton Neo-Tylorianism: Sound Sense or Sinister Prejudice? , 1968 .

[35]  Gene M. Smith Usefulness of Peer Ratings of Personality in Educational Research , 1967 .

[36]  M. Black THE RAISON D'ÊTRE OF INDUCTIVE ARGUMENT* , 1966, The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science.

[37]  S. Pepper World Hypotheses: A Study in Evidence , 1942 .

[38]  A. Tversky,et al.  BELIEF IN THE LAW OF SMALL NUMBERS , 1971, Pediatrics.

[39]  R. Horton,et al.  African Traditional Thought and Western Science , 1967, Africa.

[40]  K. Rosengren The Riddle of the Empty Cell , 1976 .

[41]  L. J. Chapman,et al.  Genesis of popular but erroneous psychodiagnostic observations. , 1967, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[42]  H. L. Raush,et al.  Interaction sequences. , 1965, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  L. J. Chapman Illusory correlation in observational report. , 1967 .

[44]  B. Bernstein Theoretical Studies Towards a Sociology of Language , 1972 .

[45]  R. Levy TAHITIANS: MIND AND EXPERIENCE IN THE SOCIETY ISLANDS by ROBERT I. LEVY. University of Chicago Press, 1973. 547 pp. $12.50. Reviewed by R. F. Salisbury , 1975 .

[46]  C. Geertz,et al.  The Interpretation of Cultures , 1973 .

[47]  L. J. Chapman,et al.  Illusory correlation as an obstacle to the use of valid psychodiagnostic signs. , 1969, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[48]  M. Black Reasoning with Loose Concepts , 1963, Dialogue.

[49]  T. Pollock,et al.  A Grammar of Motives. , 1945 .

[50]  R. Cattell Personality and motivation structure and measurement , 1958 .

[51]  M. Beckner,et al.  The biological way of thought , 1959 .

[52]  J. Smedslund THE CONCEPT OF CORRELATION IN ADULTS , 1963 .

[53]  S. Amano,et al.  Psychology of Reasoning , 1923 .

[54]  L. Ross The Intuitive Psychologist And His Shortcomings: Distortions in the Attribution Process1 , 1977 .

[55]  L. Kirk,et al.  meaning and context: a study of contextual shifts in meaning of Maasai personality descriptors , 1977 .

[56]  R R Sokal,et al.  Classification: Purposes, Principles, Progress, Prospects , 1974, Science.

[57]  T. Newcomb The consistency of certain extrovert-introvert behavior patterns in 51 problem boys. , 1929 .

[58]  W. Estes The cognitive side of probability learning. , 1976 .

[59]  A. J. Cain,et al.  LOGIC AND MEMORY IN LINNAEUS'S SYSTEM OF TAXONOMY. , 1958 .

[60]  E. Durkheim FROM THE ELEMENTARY FORMS OF THE RELIGIOUS LIFE , 1996, The New Economic Sociology.

[61]  T. Parsons,et al.  The Structure of Social Action , 1938 .

[62]  H. Kelley,et al.  Social interaction basis of cooperators' and competitors' beliefs about others. , 1970 .

[63]  E. Goffman Frame analysis: An essay on the organization of experience , 1974 .

[64]  J. Singer,et al.  Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. , 1962, Psychological review.

[65]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[66]  R. Moos Sources of variance in responses to questionnaires and in behavior. , 1969, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[67]  Eugene Borgida,et al.  Attribution and the psychology of prediction. , 1975 .

[68]  G. Āllport Personality: A Psychological Interpretation , 1938 .

[69]  Warren Weaver Lady Luck: The Theory of Probability , 1977 .

[70]  A. Kimball Romney,et al.  SIBLING TERMINOLOGY AND CROSS-SEX BEHAVIOR. , 1967 .

[71]  H. Garfinkel Studies in Ethnomethodology , 1968 .

[72]  R. H. Walters The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence , 1960 .

[73]  B. Malinowski,et al.  Magic, science and religion , 1954 .

[74]  M. Sahlins Culture and Practical Reason , 1979 .

[75]  H. Hollingworth Personality a psychological interpretation. , 1938 .

[76]  Charles O. Frake,et al.  The ethnographic study of cognitive systems , 1962 .

[77]  Rodney Needham,et al.  Polythetic Classfication: Convergence and Consequences. , 1975 .