Affect-Laden Imagery and Risk Taking: The Mediating Role of Stress and Risk Perception

This paper investigates how affect-laden imagery that evokes emotional stress influences risk perception and risk taking in real-life scenarios. In a series of three studies, we instructed participants to imagine the consequences of risky scenarios and then rate the intensity of the experienced stress, perceived risk and their willingness to engage in risky behavior. Study 1 showed that people spontaneously imagine negative rather than positive risk consequences, which are directly related to their lower willingness to take risk. Moreover, this relationship was mediated by feelings of stress and risk perception. Study 2 replicated and extended these findings by showing that imagining negative risk consequences evokes psychophysiological stress responses observed in elevated blood pressure. Finally, in Study 3, we once again demonstrated that a higher intensity of mental images of negative risk consequences, as measured by enhanced brain activity in the parieto-occipital lobes, leads to a lower propensity to take risk. Furthermore, individual differences in creating vivid and intense negative images of risk consequences moderated the strength of the relationship between risk perception and risk taking. Participants who created more vivid and intense images of negative risk consequences paid less attention to the assessments of riskiness in rating their likelihood to take risk. To summarize, we showed that feelings of emotional stress and perceived riskiness mediate the relationship between mental imagery and risk taking, whereas individual differences in abilities to create vivid mental images may influence the degree to which more cognitive risk assessments are used in the risk-taking process.

[1]  Jakub Traczyk,et al.  Implicit attitudes toward risk: the construction and validation of the measurement method , 2016 .

[2]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[3]  Lucas S. Broster,et al.  Neural Basis of Emotional Decision Making in Trait Anxiety , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  A. Hayes Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach , 2013 .

[5]  Elinor Amit,et al.  You See, the Ends Don’t Justify the Means , 2012, Psychological science.

[6]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Advantages of Monte Carlo Confidence Intervals for Indirect Effects , 2012 .

[7]  R. Schaefer,et al.  Music perception and imagery in EEG: alpha band effects of task and stimulus. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[8]  A. Baum,et al.  The Handbook of Stress Science: Biology, Psychology, and Health , 2010 .

[9]  M. Hogan,et al.  Emotion, Stress, and Cardiovascular Response: An Experimental Test of Models of Positive and Negative Affect , 2010, International journal of behavioral medicine.

[10]  Andrew F. Hayes,et al.  Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[11]  Gelder van J.-L,et al.  Evaluating a dual-process model of risk: affect and cognition as determinants of risky choice , 2009 .

[12]  R. Baayen,et al.  Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .

[13]  Valerie F Reyna,et al.  Explaining Contradictory Relations Between Risk Perception and Risk Taking , 2008, Psychological science.

[14]  Paul Slovic,et al.  The affect heuristic , 2007, Eur. J. Oper. Res..

[15]  P. Montague,et al.  Vividness of mental imagery: Individual variability can be measured objectively , 2007, Vision Research.

[16]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk — Source link , 2007 .

[17]  Tomasz Zaleskiewicz,et al.  When Does Information about Probability Count in Choices Under Risk? , 2006, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[18]  P. Slovic,et al.  Risk Perception and Affect , 2006 .

[19]  R. Hertwig,et al.  The priority heuristic: making choices without trade-offs. , 2006, Psychological review.

[20]  E. Holmes,et al.  Mental imagery and emotion: a special relationship? , 2005, Emotion.

[21]  A. Bechara The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[22]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[23]  Christopher K. Hsee,et al.  Music, Pandas, and Muggers: On the Affective Psychology of Value , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  D. Kahneman A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[25]  Mark S. Cohen,et al.  Simultaneous EEG and fMRI of the alpha rhythm , 2002, Neuroreport.

[26]  E. Weber,et al.  A Domain-Specific Risk-Attitude Scale: Measuring Risk Perceptions and Risk Behaviors , 2002 .

[27]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Rapid Processing of Monetary Gains and Losses , 2002, Science.

[28]  J. Gualberto Cremades The effects of imagery perspective as a function of skill level on alpha activity. , 2002, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[29]  Tomasz Zaleskiewicz,et al.  Beyond risk seeking and risk aversion: personality and the dual nature of economic risk taking , 2001 .

[30]  Christopher K. Hsee,et al.  Money, Kisses, and Electric Shocks: On the Affective Psychology of Risk , 2001, Psychological science.

[31]  Prashant Bordia,et al.  Assessing alternative models of individualism and collectivism: a confirmatory factor analysis , 2001 .

[32]  Christopher K. Hsee,et al.  Risk as Feelings , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. , 2001, Psychological review.

[34]  P. Slovic,et al.  Violence Risk Assessment and Risk Communication: The Effects of Using Actual Cases, Providing Instruction, and Employing Probability Versus Frequency Formats , 2000, Law and human behavior.

[35]  Paul Slovic,et al.  Imagery, Affect, and Financial Judgment , 2000 .

[36]  W. Klimesch EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis , 1999, Brain Research Reviews.

[37]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Anger and frontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  P. Slovic,et al.  Imagery, Affect, and Decision Making , 1998 .

[39]  M. F. Luce,et al.  Choice processing in emotionally difficult decisions. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  P. Slovic,et al.  The Role of Affect and Worldviews as Orienting Dispositions in the Perception and Acceptance of Nuclear Power1 , 1996 .

[41]  Stephen M. Kosslyn On Cognitive Neuroscience , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  L. Bang,et al.  24-h ambulatory blood pressure in 352 normal Danish subjects, related to age and gender. , 1995, American journal of hypertension.

[43]  S. Kosslyn Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery Debate , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  Sean A. Spence,et al.  Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain , 1995 .

[45]  D. F. Marks,et al.  Topographical distribution of EEG activity accompanying visual and motor imagery in vivid and non-vivid imagers. , 1995, British journal of psychology.

[46]  S. Epstein Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. , 1994, The American psychologist.

[47]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[48]  P Slovic,et al.  Perceived Risk, Trust, and the Politics of Nuclear Waste , 1991, Science.

[49]  M. Farah Is visual imagery really visual? Overlooked evidence from neuropsychology. , 1988, Psychological review.

[50]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[51]  R. Davidson EEG measures of cerebral asymmetry: conceptual and methodological issues. , 1988, The International journal of neuroscience.

[52]  P. Slovic Perception of risk. , 1987, Science.

[53]  Lola L. Lopes,et al.  [Advances in Experimental Social Psychology] Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Volume 20 Volume 20 || Between Hope and Fear: The Psychology of Risk , 1987 .

[54]  Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al.  A left hemisphere basis for visual mental imagery? , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[55]  W. Ray,et al.  EEG alpha activity reflects attentional demands, and beta activity reflects emotional and cognitive processes. , 1985, Science.

[56]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Individual differences in relative hemispheric alpha abundance and cognitive responses to persuasive communications. , 1982, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[57]  R. Zajonc Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferences , 1980 .

[58]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect Theory : An Analysis of Decision under Risk Author ( s ) : , 2007 .

[59]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .

[60]  R. Davidson,et al.  Brain mechanisms subserving self-generated imagery: electrophysiological specificity and patterning. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[61]  D. F. Marks,et al.  Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. , 1973, British journal of psychology.

[62]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[63]  C. Spielberger,et al.  Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory , 1970 .

[64]  E. Rowland Theory of Games and Economic Behavior , 1946, Nature.