The predation costs of symmetrical cryptic coloration

In psychological studies of visual perception, symmetry is accepted as a potent cue in visual search for cryptic objects, yet its importance for non-human animals has been assumed rather than tested. Furthermore, while the salience of bilateral symmetry has been established in laboratory-based search tasks using human subjects, its role in more natural settings, closer to those for which such perceptual mechanisms evolved, has not, to our knowledge, been investigated previously. That said, the salience of symmetry in visual search has a plausible adaptive rationale, because biologically important objects, such as prey, predators or conspecifics, usually have a plane of symmetry that is not present in their surroundings. We tested the conspicuousness to avian predators of cryptic artificial, moth-like targets, with or without bilateral symmetry in background-matching coloration, against oak trees in the field. In two independent experiments, symmetrical targets were predated at a higher rate than otherwise identical asymmetrical targets. There was a small, but significant, fitness cost to symmetry in camouflage patterns. Given that birds are the most commonly invoked predators shaping the evolution of defensive coloration in insects, this raises the question of why bilateral asymmetry is not more common in cryptic insects.

[1]  S. Carroll,et al.  Pattern formation and eyespot determination in butterfly wings. , 1994, Science.

[2]  G. Ruxton,et al.  Avoiding Attack: The Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Warning Signals and Mimicry , 2004 .

[3]  Rick Gurnsey,et al.  Bilateral symmetry embedded in noise is detected accurately only at fixation , 1998, Vision Research.

[4]  Stalker,et al.  A bird’s eye view of the peppered moth , 2000 .

[5]  R. Dilão,et al.  Modelling butterfly wing eyespot patterns , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[6]  J. Klein,et al.  Survival Analysis: Techniques for Censored and Truncated Data , 1997 .

[7]  H. Barlow,et al.  The versatility and absolute efficiency of detecting mirror symmetry in random dot displays , 1979, Vision Research.

[8]  Stephane J. M. Rainville,et al.  Spatial-scale contribution to the detection of mirror symmetry in fractal noise. , 1999, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[9]  A. Thayer The Law Which Underlies Protective Coloration , 1896 .

[10]  A. Pomiankowski,et al.  Fluctuating asymmetry and sexual selection , 1993, Genetica.

[11]  I. Cuthill,et al.  Plumage Reflectance and the Objective Assessment of Avian Sexual Dichromatism , 1999, The American Naturalist.

[12]  S. Merilaita,et al.  Fearful symmetry: pattern size and asymmetry affects aposematic signal efficacy , 1999, Evolutionary Ecology.

[13]  J. Endler An overview of the relationships between mimicry and crypsis , 1981 .

[14]  S. Merilaita,et al.  Fearful symmetry? Intra-individual comparisons of asymmetry in cryptic vs. signalling colour patterns in butterflies , 2003, Evolutionary Ecology.

[15]  M. Huynen,et al.  Disruptive coloration and background pattern matching , 2005, Nature.

[16]  D.,et al.  Regression Models and Life-Tables , 2022 .

[17]  H. B. Cott,et al.  Adaptive Coloration in Animals , 1940 .

[18]  H. Nijhout,et al.  The development and evolution of butterfly wing patterns , 1991 .

[19]  J. Endler A Predator’s View of Animal Color Patterns , 1978 .

[20]  A. Thayer,et al.  Concealing-coloration in the animal kingdom : an exposition of the laws of disguise through color and pattern being a summary of Abbott H. Thayer's discoveries , 1909 .

[21]  I. Cuthill,et al.  Ultraviolet Vision in Birds , 2000 .

[22]  A. Møller,et al.  Asymmetry, Developmental Stability, and Evolution , 1998 .

[23]  Gordon Johnston,et al.  Statistical Models and Methods for Lifetime Data , 2003, Technometrics.

[24]  V. French,et al.  The relationship between eyespot shape and wing shape in the butterfly Bicyclus anynana: A genetic and morphometrical approach , 1997 .

[25]  S. Merilaita Crypsis through disruptive coloration in an isopod , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[26]  R. Menzel,et al.  Symmetry perception in an insect , 1996, Nature.

[27]  Jerald F. Lawless,et al.  Statistical Models and Methods for Lifetime Data: Lawless/Statistical , 2002 .

[28]  E. Hancock,et al.  Asymmetrical colour and wing-folding in Tithrone roseipennis (Saussure 1870) a neotropical praying mantis (Mantodea Hymenopodidae) , 1999 .

[29]  D. Osorlo Symmetry versus crypsis , 1994 .

[30]  Sean B. Carroll,et al.  "Development, Plasticity and Evolution of Butterfly Eyespot Patterns" (1996), by Paul M. Brakefield et al. , 2013 .

[31]  R. Behrens False Colors: Art, Design and Modern Camouflage , 2002 .

[32]  V. French,et al.  The relationship between eyespot shape and wing shape in the butterfly , 1997 .

[33]  J. Lythgoe The Ecology of vision , 1979 .

[34]  J. Endler Progressive background in moths, and a quantitative measure of crypsis , 1984 .