Visual working memory load does not eliminate visuomotor repetition effects
暂无分享,去创建一个
Jay Pratt | Jason Rajsic | Matthew D. Hilchey | Geoffrey F Woodman | Matthew D Hilchey | G. Woodman | J. Pratt | Jason Rajsic
[1] G. Woodman,et al. Visual search is slowed when visuospatial working memory is occupied , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[2] Matthew D. Hilchey,et al. Feature integration in basic detection and localization tasks: Insights from the attentional orienting literature , 2018, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[3] Denis G. Pelli,et al. ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.
[4] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Processing of representations in declarative and procedural working memory , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[5] M. Chun,et al. Organization of visual short-term memory. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[6] B. Hommel,et al. Intentional weighting: a basic principle in cognitive control , 2012, Psychological research.
[7] B. Hommel,et al. Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.
[8] C. Frings,et al. Dissociation of binding and learning processes , 2017, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[9] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Consciousness is not necessary for visual feature binding , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[10] P. Bertelson,et al. Serial Choice Reaction-time as a Function of Response versus Signal-and-Response Repetition , 1965, Nature.
[11] Arni Kristjánsson,et al. The boundary conditions of priming of visual search: From passive viewing through task-relevant working memory load , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[12] B. Hommel,et al. A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task , 2004, Psychological research.
[13] Daryl E. Wilson,et al. Long-term facilitation of return: A response-retrieval effect , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[14] Matthew D. Hilchey,et al. Response-mediated spatial priming despite perfectly valid target location cues and intervening response events , 2017 .
[15] B. Hommel,et al. What do we learn from binding features? Evidence for multilevel feature integration. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. How to measure working memory capacity in the change detection paradigm , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[17] Matthew D. Hilchey,et al. Dissociating Orienting Biases From Integration Effects With Eye Movements , 2018, Psychological science.
[18] Alessandra S. Souza,et al. Interference within and between declarative and procedural representations in working memory , 2014 .
[19] C. Bundesen. A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.
[20] Andrea Kiesel,et al. New perspectives on human multitasking , 2018, Psychological Research.
[21] Matthew D. Hilchey,et al. Is attention really biased toward the last target location in visual search? Attention, response rules, distractors, and eye movements , 2019, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[22] B. Hommel. How much attention does an event file need? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[23] Jay Pratt,et al. The role of spatial working memory in inhibition of return: Evidence from divided attention tasks , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[24] Harold Pashler,et al. Procedural learning: II. Intertrial repetition effects in speeded-choice tasks. , 1991 .
[25] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[26] B. Hommel. Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[27] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of popout: III. A short-term implicit memory system beneficial for rapid target selection , 2000 .
[28] J. Brockmole,et al. Bound feature combinations in visual short-term memory are fragile but influence long-term learning , 2009 .
[29] Yuhong Jiang,et al. Proactive interference from items previously stored in visual working memory , 2008, Memory & cognition.
[30] S. Luck,et al. Proactive Interference Does Not Meaningfully Distort Visual Working Memory Capacity Estimates in the Canonical Change Detection Task , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[31] Darryl W. Schneider,et al. A memory-based model of Hick’s law , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.
[32] G. Woodman,et al. Selective storage and maintenance of an object’s features in visual working memory , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[33] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Theory of Event Coding (TEC) V2.0: Representing and controlling perception and action , 2019, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[34] B. Hommel,et al. Attentional control of the creation and retrieval of stimulus–response bindings , 2014, Psychological research.
[35] Hyunkyu Lee,et al. Mechanisms of priming of pop-out: Stored representations or feature-gain modulations? , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[36] Motonori Yamaguchi,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility with pure and mixed mappings in a flight task environment. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[37] J. de Houwer,et al. Retrieval of incidental stimulus-response associations as a source of negative priming. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[38] Nancy B. Carlisle,et al. How visual working memory contents influence priming of visual attention , 2018, Psychological research.
[39] Edward K. Vogel,et al. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions , 1997, Nature.
[40] Yang Zhang,et al. Spatial working memory load impairs manual but not saccadic inhibition of return , 2011, Vision Research.
[41] B. Hommel. Event Files: Evidence for Automatic Integration of Stimulus-Response Episodes , 1998 .
[42] Trisha N Patel,et al. Exploring the contributions of spatial and non-spatial working memory to priming of pop-out , 2017, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[43] B. Hommel,et al. Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration , 2004 .
[44] M. Posner,et al. Components of visual orienting , 1984 .
[45] N. Cowan. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[46] Edward K Vogel,et al. Flexibility in visual working memory: Accurate change detection in the face of irrelevant variations in position , 2012, Visual cognition.
[47] Zachary J. J. Roper,et al. Visual short-term memory load strengthens selective attention , 2014, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[48] R. Henson,et al. Stimulus–response bindings in priming , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[49] T Kleinsorge,et al. Response repetition benefits and costs. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[50] G. Logan,et al. On the Use of a Concurrent Memory Load to Measure Attention and Automaticity , 1979 .
[51] D. Kahneman,et al. The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.
[52] G. Logan. Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.
[53] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Design for a working memory. , 2009 .
[54] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Do stimulus–response bindings survive a task switch? , 2006 .
[55] Matthew D. Hilchey,et al. Intervening response events between identification targets do not always turn repetition benefits into repetition costs , 2017, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[56] S W Keele,et al. Processing demands of sequential information , 1973, Memory & cognition.
[57] Daryl E. Wilson,et al. Long-Term Inhibition of Return for Spatial Locations: Evidence for a Memory Retrieval Account , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[58] Joshua K. Hartshorne. Visual Working Memory Capacity and Proactive Interference , 2008, PloS one.
[59] James R. Schmidt,et al. The Parallel Episodic Processing (PEP) model 2.0: A single computational model of stimulus-response binding, contingency learning, power curves, and mixing costs , 2016, Cognitive Psychology.
[60] B. Hommel,et al. When an object is more than a binding of its features: Evidence for two mechanisms of visual feature integration , 2009 .
[61] David W. Sutterer,et al. The Role of Long-Term Memory in a Test of Visual Working Memory: Proactive Facilitation but No Proactive Interference , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.