Ecological constraints on internal representation: resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming.

ness of Internalized Perceptual Constraints The two-dimensional case of Chasles's theorem provides the simplest illustration of the abstractness of the internalized constraints. From considerations of physical dynamics, one might guess that two planar figures alternately presented in positions that differ arbitrarily (and hence by both a translation and a rotation, as in Panel f of Figure 2) would give rise to an apparent motion in which the center of mass of the apparently moving body traverses the shortest, straight line between its two terminal positions. Because the two views also differ by a rotation, such a motion would have to be accompanied by an additional, apparent rotational transformation, as illustrated for two rectangles in Figure 3, Part a. Instead of such a double transformation, however, Foster (1975) found Figure 3. Intermediate positions of a rectangle (drawn in thin lines) between the same two rectangles (drawn in heavy lines), which differ arbitrarily in both position and orientation, along a path consisting of a combined rectilinear translation and a rotation (Part a), and the path (which Foster, 1975, found to be preferred in apparent motion) consisting of a rotation only (Part b). (From Mental Images and Their Transformations by R. N. Shepard and L. A. Cooper, 1982, p. 316. Copyright 1982 by The Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Adapted by permission.) that the motion is generally experienced over a curved path. By having observers adjust the variable intermediate rectangle (indicated in Figure 3 by thinner lines) so that it appeared to fall on the path of motion, he found that (under conducive conditions) the motion tended to be experienced over that unique circular path that rigidly carries the one figure into the other by a single rotation about a fixed point, P, in the plane, as shown in Figure 3, Part b. It seems that here, as in the case of the moire pattern of Glass (1969; an example of which is shown in Figure 4, Part b), the visual system picks out the fixed point implied by the two presented positions of a rigid configuration in the plane and, hence, identifies the two configurations with each other by means of a simple rotation. (See Foster, 1975, 1978; Shepard, 1981b; and for a review and theoretical discussion, Shepard & Cooper, 4 In an investigation of apparent motion motivated by similar objectives, Warren (1977) reported that alternation between two-dimensional shapes differing by an affine transformation did not yield rigid apparent motion. However his allegedly affine pair (Panel g) was not affine, and his instructions and resulting subjective reports are open to questions of interpretation, choice of criterion, and effects of perceptual set or expectancy. 426 ROGER N. SHEPARD Figure 4. Moire pattern described by Glass (1969), in which two identical transparencies of a random texture (Part a), when superimposed in an arbitrary misalignment, give rise to the appearance of concentric circles (Part b). (As one transparency is shifted with respect to the other, the center of the concentric circles moves in an orthogonal direction.) 1982.) Incidentally, the visual system also extracts fixed points in the case of nonrigid transformations, as has been demonstrated by Johansson (1950, 1973), Wallach (1965/ 1976), and most extensively by Cutting and his associates (see Cutting, 1981; Cutting & Proffitt, 1982). There are good reasons why the automatic operations of the perceptual system should be guided more by general principles of kinematic geometry than by specific principles governing the different probable behaviors of particular objects. Chasles's theorem constrains the motion of each semirigid part of a body, during each moment of time, to a simple, six-degrees-of-freedom twisting motion, including the limiting cases of pure rotations or translations. By contrast, the more protracted motions of particular objects (a falling leaf, floating stick, diving bird, or pouncing cat) have vastly more degrees of freedom that respond quite differently to many unknowable factors (breezes, currents, memories, or intentions). Moreover, relative to a rapidly moving observer, the spatial transformations of even nonrigid, insubstantial, or transient objects (snakes, bushes, waves, clouds, or wisps of smoke) behave like the transformations of rigid objects (Shepard & Cooper, 1982). It is not surprising then that the automatic perceptual impletion that is revealed in apparent motion does not attempt either the impossible prediction or the arbitrary selection of one natural motion out of the many appropriate to the particular object. Rather, it simply instantiates the continuing existence of the object by means of the unique, simplest rigid motion that will carry the one view into the other, and it does so in a way that is compatible with a movement either of the observer or of the object observed. Possibly some pervasive principles of physical dynamics (such as a principle of momentum), in addition to the more abstract principles of purely kinematic geometry, have been internalized to the extent that they influence apparent motion (Foster & Gravano, 1982; Freyd, 1983a,. 1983c, 1983d, 1983e; Freyd & Finke, 1984; Ramachandran & Anstis, 1983). But there evidently is little or no effect of the particular object presented. The motion we involuntarily experience when a picture of an object is presented first in one place and then in another, whether the picture is of a leaf or of a cat, is neither a fluttering drift nor a pounce; it is, in both cases, the same simplest, rigid displacement. True, we may imagine a leaf fluttering down or a cat pouncing, but in doing so we voluntarily undertake a more complex simulation (just as we might in imagining a leaf pouncing or a cat fluttering down). Such mental simulations may be guided by internalizations of more specific principles of physical dynamics and even perhaps of animal behavior. Pervasive Constraints of Time and Distance I have taken the sources of the perceptual constraints considered so far to be corresponding constraints in the world, for example, the 24-hour diurnal cycle and principles of kinematic geometry and perhaps of physical dynamics. However, there are other highly orderly perceptual regularities that may not be reflections of constraints that happened to prevail in our world so much as manifestations of constraints that are unavoidable in any system that could exist in this world. Thus, much as the velocity of light limits the speed of communication between distant bodies, the necessarily finite velocity of signal ECOLOGICAL CONSTRAINTS ON INTERNAL REPRESENTATION 427

[1]  Familiar lectures on scientific subjects , 1867 .

[2]  J. D. Everett A Treatise on the Theory of Screws , 1901, Nature.

[3]  New York Association for the Blind , 1907 .

[4]  R. Woodworth,et al.  A revision of imageless thought. , 1915 .

[5]  I. Krechevsky,et al.  "Hypotheses" in rats. , 1932 .

[6]  G. Hall,et al.  The Interpretation of Dreams , 1914, Nature.

[7]  N. Rashevsky Mathematical Biophysics , 1935, Nature.

[8]  J. F. Brown,et al.  The Path of Seen Movement as a Function of the Vector-Field , 1937 .

[9]  Jesse Orlansky,et al.  The effect of similarity and difference in form on apparent visual movement , 1940 .

[10]  H. H. Corbin The perception of grouping and apparent movement in visual depth. , 1942 .

[11]  E. Cassirer The concept of group and the theory of perception , 1944 .

[12]  J. Hadamard,et al.  The Psychology of Invention in the Mathematical Field. , 1945 .

[13]  I. Richards,et al.  The Portable Coleridge , 1950 .

[14]  S. Gunnar O. Johansson,et al.  Configurations in event perception : an experimental study , 1951 .

[15]  A. Ames Visual perception and the rotating trapezoidal window , 1951 .

[16]  R. Hetherington The Perception of the Visual World , 1952 .

[17]  H. H. Price Thinking and representation , 1953 .

[18]  H. Wallach,et al.  The kinetic depth effect. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.

[19]  J. Gibson,et al.  Perceptual learning; differentiation or enrichment? , 1955, Psychological review.

[20]  F ATTNEAVE,et al.  The quantitative study of shape and pattern perception. , 1956, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  U. T. Place,et al.  Is consciousness a brain process? , 1956, British journal of psychology.

[22]  E. Brunswik Perception and the Representative Design of Psychological Experiments , 1957 .

[23]  J. Bruner On perceptual readiness. , 1957, Psychological review.

[24]  W. Penfield The Excitable Cortex in Conscious Man , 1958 .

[25]  J. J. C. Smart,et al.  Sensations and brain processes , 1959 .

[26]  W. Pitts,et al.  What the Frog's Eye Tells the Frog's Brain , 1959, Proceedings of the IRE.

[27]  P. Squires Topological aspects of apparent visual motion , 1959, Psychologische Forschung.

[28]  Imprinting. , 1960, Science.

[29]  Kenneth L. Artis Design for a Brain , 1961 .

[30]  B. Green Figure coherence in the kinetic depth effect. , 1961, Journal of experimental psychology.

[31]  Peter H. Greene,et al.  On looking for neural networks and "cell assemblies" that underlie behavior. II. Neural realization of the mathematical model. , 1962 .

[32]  P H GREENE On looking for neural networks and "cell assemblies" that underlie behavior. I. A mathematical model. , 1962, The Bulletin of mathematical biophysics.

[33]  Hermann von Helmholtz,et al.  Treatise on Physiological Optics , 1962 .

[34]  J. Austin,et al.  Sense and Sensibilia , 1963 .

[35]  H. Coxeter,et al.  Introduction to Geometry , 1964, The Mathematical Gazette.

[36]  Erwin Bünning,et al.  The Physiological Clock , 1964, Heidelberg Science Library.

[37]  Ewald Hering Outlines of a theory of the light sense , 1964 .

[38]  R. Shepard Circularity in Judgments of Relative Pitch , 1964 .

[39]  A. Michael Noll,et al.  COMPUTER - GENERATED THREE-DIMENSIONAL MOVIES , 1965 .

[40]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .

[41]  D. T. Greenwood Principles of dynamics , 1965 .

[42]  D H HUBEL,et al.  RECEPTIVE FIELDS AND FUNCTIONAL ARCHITECTURE IN TWO NONSTRIATE VISUAL AREAS (18 AND 19) OF THE CAT. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  J. Hay,et al.  Optical motions and space perception: an extension of Gibson's analysis. , 1966, Psychological review.

[44]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[45]  U. Neisser Cognitive Psychology: Classic Edition , 1967 .

[46]  M. Scheerer,et al.  Problem Solving , 1967, Nature.

[47]  J. Hochberg,et al.  Recognition of faces: I. An exploratory study , 1967 .

[48]  J. Konorski Integrative activity of the brain , 1967 .

[49]  R W Sperry,et al.  Hemisphere deconnection and unity in conscious awareness. , 1968, The American psychologist.

[50]  L. Glass Moiré Effect from Random Dots , 1969, Nature.

[51]  R. Yin Looking at Upside-down Faces , 1969 .

[52]  M. Gazzaniga The Bisected Brain , 1970 .

[53]  James J. Gibson,et al.  On the Relation between Hallucination and Perception , 1970 .

[54]  R. M. Warren,et al.  HELMHOLTZ ON PERCEPTION: ITS PHYSIOLOGY AND DEVELOPMENT. , 1970 .

[55]  Michael I. Posner,et al.  Abstraction and The Process of Recognition , 1970 .

[56]  R. Shepard,et al.  Second-order isomorphism of internal representations: Shapes of states ☆ , 1970 .

[57]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[58]  P A Kolers,et al.  Figural change in apparent motion. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[59]  James J. Gibson,et al.  The Information Available in Pictures , 1971 .

[60]  Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development , 1971 .

[61]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in inferotemporal cortex of the Macaque. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[62]  P. A. Kolers Aspects of motion perception , 1972 .

[63]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[64]  F. Attneave,et al.  Apparent movement in tridimensional space , 1973 .

[65]  E. Gibson,et al.  Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development , 1973 .

[66]  James J. Gibson,et al.  Visualizing Conceived as Visual Apprehending without any Particular Point of Observation , 1974 .

[67]  Fred Attneave,et al.  Apparent movement and the what-where connection. , 1974 .

[68]  R. Shepard,et al.  Transformational studies of the internal representation of three-dimensional objects. , 1974 .

[69]  J. Wheeler,et al.  The Physics of Time Asymmetry , 1974 .

[70]  G. Johansson Visual motion perception. , 1975, Scientific American.

[71]  L. V. Noorden Temporal coherence in the perception of tone sequences , 1975 .

[72]  I. Zucker,et al.  Biological rhythms and animal behavior. , 1975, Annual review of psychology.

[73]  J. B. Pittenger,et al.  Aging faces as viscal-elastic events: implications for a theory of nonrigid shape perception. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[74]  L. Cooper Mental rotation of random two-dimensional shapes , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[75]  David L. Waltz,et al.  Understanding Line drawings of Scenes with Shadows , 1975 .

[76]  Donald O. Walter,et al.  Mass action in the nervous system , 1975 .

[77]  D. Navon,et al.  Irrelevance of figural identity for resolving ambiguities in apparent motion. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[78]  L. Cooper Demonstration of a mental analog of an external rotation , 1976 .

[79]  L. Cooper,et al.  Mental transformations and visual comparison processes: effects of complexity and similarity. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[80]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .

[81]  R. Shepard,et al.  Perceptual illusion of rotation of three-dimensional objects. , 1976, Science.

[82]  M. S. Mayzner,et al.  Cognition And Reality , 1976 .

[83]  M. Braunstein Depth perception through motion , 1976 .

[84]  M. R. Jones,et al.  Time, our lost dimension: toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. , 1976, Psychological review.

[85]  Stephen A. Ritz,et al.  Distinctive features, categorical perception, and probability learning: some applications of a neural model , 1977 .

[86]  Roger N. Shepard,et al.  Spatio-temporal probing of apparent rotational movement , 1977 .

[87]  William H. Warren,et al.  Visual information for object identity in apparent movement , 1977 .

[88]  J. Cutting,et al.  Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display , 1977 .

[89]  Robert Rosen,et al.  Structural stability and morphogenesis , 1977 .

[90]  Ulric Neisser,et al.  Gibson' s Ecological Optics: Consequences of a Different Stimulus Description* , 1977 .

[91]  S. Runeson On the possibility of "smart" perceptual mechanisms. , 1977, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[92]  Roger N. Shepard,et al.  TRANSFORMATIONS ON REPRESENTATIONS OF OBJECTS IN SPACE , 1978 .

[93]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Unilateral Neglect of Representational Space , 1978, Cortex.

[94]  K. H. Hunt,et al.  Kinematic geometry of mechanisms , 1978 .

[95]  T Caelli,et al.  Subjective Lorentz transformations and the perception of motion. , 1978, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[96]  D. Foster Visual apparent motion and the calculus of variations , 1978 .

[97]  R. Shepard,et al.  Functional representations common to visual perception and imagination. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[98]  A. D. D. Groot Thought and Choice in Chess , 1978 .

[99]  E. Gibson,et al.  Perception of Invariants by Five-Month-Old Infants: Differentiation of Two Types of Motion. , 1978 .

[100]  R. Shepard The mental image. , 1978 .

[101]  J E Cutting,et al.  A biomechanical invariant for gait perception. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[102]  J E Cutting,et al.  Perceiving the centroid of configurations on a rolling wheel , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[103]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[104]  S. Ullman The Interpretation of Visual Motion , 1979 .

[105]  F. Restle Coding theory of the perception of motion configurations. , 1979, Psychological review.

[106]  R. Finke Levels of equivalence in imagery and perception. , 1980 .

[107]  S. Ullman Against direct perception , 1980, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[108]  Samuel Shye,et al.  Theory construction and data analysis in the behavioral sciences , 1980 .

[109]  S. Grossberg,et al.  How does a brain build a cognitive code? , 1980, Psychological review.

[110]  Gerald J. Balzano,et al.  The group-theoretic description of 12-fold and microtonal pitch systems , 1980 .

[111]  G Jansson,et al.  Event perception. , 1980, Annual review of psychology.

[112]  R N Shepard,et al.  Shape, orientation, and apparent rotational motion. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[113]  R. Harré Great Scientific Experiments: Twenty Experiments that Changed our View of the World , 1981 .

[114]  F. Keil Constraints on knowledge and cognitive development. , 1981 .

[115]  Roger N. Shepard,et al.  Psychological relations and psychophysical scales: On the status of “direct” psychophysical measurement ☆ , 1981 .

[116]  J. Hochberg On cognition in perception: Perceptual coupling and unconscious inference , 1981, Cognition.

[117]  J. Cutting Six tenets for event perception , 1981, Cognition.

[118]  D. Foster,et al.  Overshoot of curvature in visual apparent motion , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[119]  C. Bundesen,et al.  Velocity Constraints on Apparent Rotational Movement , 1982, Perception.

[120]  S. Kosslyn Image and mind , 1982 .

[121]  J. Cutting,et al.  The minimum principle and the perception of absolute, common, and relative motions , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[122]  R. Shepard Geometrical approximations to the structure of musical pitch. , 1982, Psychological review.

[123]  B. Bergum,et al.  Attention and performance IX , 1982 .

[124]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Images and Their Transformations , 1982 .

[125]  M. Garrett,et al.  Perceptual knowledge of objects in infancy , 1982 .

[126]  M. Kubovy Mental imagery majestically transforming cognitive psychology. , 1983 .

[127]  R. Shepard,et al.  Distribution of visual attention over space. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[128]  J. Freyd The mental representation of action , 1983, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[129]  N. Mai,et al.  Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[130]  E. Gibson,et al.  The development of perception , 1983 .

[131]  J. Kirman,et al.  Tactile apparent movement: The effects of shape and type of motion , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[132]  J. Farrell,et al.  Visual transformations underlying apparent movement , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[133]  J. Thomson Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[134]  JENNIFER J. FREYD,et al.  Shareability: The Social Psychology of Epistemology , 1983, Cogn. Sci..

[135]  Roger N. Shepard,et al.  Demonstrations of Circular Components of Pitch , 1983 .

[136]  J. Freyd,et al.  The mental representation of movement when static stimuli are viewed , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[137]  J. Freyd,et al.  Representing the dynamics of a static form , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[138]  Joseph Ford,et al.  How random is a coin toss , 1983 .

[139]  R N Shepard,et al.  Path-guided apparent motion. , 1983, Science.

[140]  C Bundesen,et al.  Visual Apparent Movement: Transformations of Size and Orientation , 1983, Perception.

[141]  J. Freyd,et al.  A velocity effect for representational momentum , 1985 .

[142]  A. Noē Direct Perception , 2022 .