Memory in 3-month-old infants benefits from a short nap.

A broad range of studies demonstrate that sleep has a facilitating role in memory consolidation (see Rasch & Born, ). Whether sleep-dependent memory consolidation is also apparent in infants in their first few months of life has not been investigated. We demonstrate that 3-month-old infants only remember a cartoon face approximately 1.5-2 hours after its first presentation when a period of sleep followed learning. Furthermore, habituation time, that is, the time to become bored with a stimulus shown repetitively, correlated negatively with the density of infant sleep spindles, implying that processing speed is linked to specific electroencephalographic components of sleep. Our findings show that without a short period of sleep infants have problems remembering a newly seen face, that sleep enhances memory consolidation from a very early age, highlighting the importance of napping in infancy, and that infant sleep spindles may be associated with some aspects of cognitive ability.

[1]  Denise M Werchan,et al.  Sleep confers a benefit for retention of statistical language learning in 6.5month old infants , 2017, Brain and Language.

[2]  Ferenc Gombos,et al.  Sleep spindles and intelligence in early childhood-developmental and trait-dependent aspects. , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[3]  K. Plunkett,et al.  Frequent daytime naps predict vocabulary growth in early childhood , 2016, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[4]  Dietmar Schmitz,et al.  Cellular and System Biology of Memory: Timing, Molecules, and Beyond. , 2016, Physiological reviews.

[5]  K. Plunkett,et al.  Napping facilitates word learning in early lexical development , 2015, Journal of sleep research.

[6]  Rebecca L Gómez,et al.  Sleep as a window into early neural development: Shifts in sleep-dependent learning effects across early childhood. , 2015, Child development perspectives.

[7]  Boris N. Konrad,et al.  A comparison of two sleep spindle detection methods based on all night averages: individually adjusted vs. fixed frequencies , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[8]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  Generalization of word meanings during infant sleep , 2015, Nature Communications.

[9]  Jane S. Herbert,et al.  Timely sleep facilitates declarative memory consolidation in infants , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[10]  Adrián Pótári,et al.  Sleep Spindles and Intelligence: Evidence for a Sexual Dimorphism , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  I. Kovács,et al.  Sleep spindling and fluid intelligence across adolescent development: sex matters , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[12]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  Slow sleep spindle activity, declarative memory, and general cognitive abilities in children. , 2014, Sleep.

[13]  Eleanor A. Maguire,et al.  Learning to remember: The early ontogeny of episodic memory , 2014, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  G. Tononi,et al.  Sleep and the Price of Plasticity: From Synaptic and Cellular Homeostasis to Memory Consolidation and Integration , 2014, Neuron.

[15]  Pierre Lavenex,et al.  Building hippocampal circuits to learn and remember: Insights into the development of human memory , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[16]  Rebecca M C Spencer,et al.  Sleep spindles in midday naps enhance learning in preschool children , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[17]  J. Born,et al.  About sleep's role in memory. , 2013, Physiological reviews.

[18]  Mathias Baumert,et al.  Sleep spindle activity and cognitive performance in healthy children. , 2013, Sleep.

[19]  Peter Achermann,et al.  Triangular Relationship between Sleep Spindle Activity, General Cognitive Ability and the Efficiency of Declarative Learning , 2012, PloS one.

[20]  T. Burbacher,et al.  Measuring infant memory: Utility of the visual paired-comparison test paradigm for studies in developmental neurotoxicology. , 2012, Neurotoxicology and teratology.

[21]  Peter Achermann,et al.  The sleep EEG as a marker of intellectual ability in school age children. , 2011, Sleep.

[22]  M. Sankupellay,et al.  Characteristics of sleep EEG power spectra in healthy infants in the first two years of life , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[23]  Robert Stickgold,et al.  Sleep Spindle Activity is Associated with the Integration of New Memories and Existing Knowledge , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Maya Ringli,et al.  Mapping of Cortical Activity in the First Two Decades of Life: A High-Density Sleep Electroencephalogram Study , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  A. Hupbach,et al.  Nap-dependent learning in infants. , 2009, Developmental science.

[26]  R. Bódizs,et al.  The individual adjustment method of sleep spindle analysis: Methodological improvements and roots in the fingerprint paradigm , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[27]  H. Scholle,et al.  Sleep spindle evolution from infancy to adolescence , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[28]  R. Chervin,et al.  The visual scoring of sleep and arousal in infants and children. , 2007, Journal of clinical sleep medicine : JCSM : official publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine.

[29]  Rebecca L. Gómez,et al.  Naps Promote Abstraction in Language-Learning Infants , 2006, Psychological science.

[30]  D. Fabó,et al.  Twenty-four hours retention of visuospatial memory correlates with the number of parietal sleep spindles , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[31]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  Sleep spindle‐related activity in the human EEG and its relation to general cognitive and learning abilities , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[32]  D. Fabó,et al.  Overnight verbal memory retention correlates with the number of sleep spindles , 2005, Neuroscience.

[33]  Mario Rosanova,et al.  Pattern-Specific Associative Long-Term Potentiation Induced by a Sleep Spindle-Related Spike Train , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  Z. Clemens,et al.  Prediction of general mental ability based on neural oscillation measures of sleep , 2005, Journal of sleep research.

[35]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation. , 2004, Sleep.

[36]  P. Achermann,et al.  Development of the nocturnal sleep electroencephalogram in human infants. , 2004, American journal of physiology. Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology.

[37]  J. Born,et al.  Learning-Dependent Increases in Sleep Spindle Density , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  M. Steriade Coherent oscillations and short-term plasticity in corticothalamic networks , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[39]  G Buzsáki,et al.  The hippocampo-neocortical dialogue. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  D. Metcalf,et al.  Sleep Spindles , 1985, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[41]  K. Plunkett,et al.  A Daytime Nap Facilitates Generalization of Word Meanings in Young Toddlers. , 2016, Sleep.

[42]  B. Nolan Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory , 2008 .

[43]  O. Pascalis,et al.  Long-term recognition memory for faces assessed by visual paired comparison in 3- and 6-month-old infants. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[44]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .

[45]  A. Slater Individual differences in infancy and later IQ. , 1995, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[46]  L. Radloff The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population — Source link , 2022 .